MIT Philosophy Logo

Department of Linguistics and Philosophy

Graduate admissions, requirements.

To enter the doctoral program , students must have done well in their previous academic work and must be formally accepted as candidates for the Ph.D. degree by the Department of Linguistics and Philosophy. Furthermore, an applicant must have received a bachelor’s degree or its equivalent from a college or university of acceptable standing.

Although there are no formal requirements for admission, successful applicants typically have an undergraduate major in philosophy, or a comparable level of preparation.

The Application

A complete application includes the MIT application form, an academic transcript from each college/university attended, and three letters of recommendation.

In addition to the application, all applicants should submit a writing sample in philosophy, ideally of 15-25 pages in length. The writing sample should allow us to assess the applicant’s understanding of a philosophical problem, and ability to evaluate philosophical arguments. This assessment is usually easier if the writing sample explicitly engages with some of the contemporary philosophical literature.

Students are only admitted into the program in the fall. The application deadline is January 2nd for the following September. Decisions are communicated to applicants by early March.

GRE scores are optional : they are  not required for admission to the doctoral program. The reporting codes are:

Institute code: 3514 Department code: 2804

MIT requires international applicants whose native language is not English to submit the results of the Test of English as a Foreign Language (TOEFL) or the International English Language Testing System (IELTS).

TOEFL: Minimum score required: 577 (PBT) 90 (IBT) TOEFL Department codes: 20 (Philosophy) Institute code: 3514 (MIT)

IELTS: Minimum score required: 6.5 Electronic scores send to: MIT Graduate Admissions

However, the Department of Linguistics and Philosophy does grant waivers. In general, we grant waivers to students who have received a degree from an American or English-speaking university, or who show an extensive background in English. We ask that students who request a TOEFL or IELTS waiver have their recommenders comment in depth on their English speaking, reading and writing skills as a part of the recommendation. We also strongly suggest that they submit a writing sample in English. Applicants may request a waiver in the application.

Accessing the Application Form

To apply, please visit: https://gradapply.mit.edu/phil

The statement of purpose, letters of recommendation and writing sample are submitted on-line. Scanned copies of transcripts are submitted on-line for review purposes but are considered unofficial. Applicants who are admitted will be required to send an official transcript in a university sealed envelope.

The application fee is $75.00 in US funds. Major credit cards are the accepted form of payment. MIT offers fee waivers to students who meet relevant criteria. For criteria and more information, please visit:  https://oge.mit.edu/graduate-admissions/applications/application-fee-waiver/ .  If you attended PIKSI or another  Undergraduate Diversity Institute in Philosophy , please contact  [email protected]  regarding a fee waiver.

Questions regarding the application process can be sent to  [email protected] .

In response to the challenges of teaching, learning, and assessing academic performance during the global COVID-19 pandemic, MIT has adopted the following principle: MIT’s admissions committees and offices for graduate and professional schools will take the significant disruptions of the COVID-19 outbreak in 2020 into account when reviewing students’ transcripts and other admissions materials as part of their regular practice of performing individualized, holistic reviews of each applicant.

In particular, as we review applications now and in the future, we will respect decisions regarding the adoption of Pass/No Record (or Credit/No Credit or Pass/Fail) and other grading options during the unprecedented period of COVID-19 disruptions, whether those decisions were made by institutions or by individual students. We also expect that the individual experiences of applicants will richly inform applications and, as such, they will be considered with the entirety of a student’s record.

Frequently Asked Questions

Do you have a masters program.

No. The philosophy graduate program is a doctoral program only. In the Boston area, Tufts University offers a masters degree in philosophy.

How long is the Ph.D. program?

Five years.

Is a master’s degree required to apply to the PhD program?

A master’s degree is not required. Prospective students may apply to the PhD program direct from their undergraduate studies.

Can I earn a doctoral degree through distance learning?

No. You can give our   MOOCs a try, though.

Do students get financial support from MIT?

Yes. Currently, we can support five entering students each year with tuition plus a 12-month stipend. We can also support graduating fifth-years with a one-year post-doc if they are unable to secure a position elsewhere. In special cases, we are able to support sixth year students with tuition plus a 9-month stipend in place of a post-doc.

Although MIT regulations prohibit us from guaranteeing funding for more than any one year, we fully expect to be able to support students in good standing throughout the entire five-year period of the doctoral program. However, the Department has limited funds for financial aid to graduate students, so we encourage you to apply for  graduate fellowships  from government agencies and foundations.

I am an international applicant. Am I still eligible for financial support from MIT?

Yes. All admitted students, whether or not they are US citizens, are eligible for support.

Does the Ph.D. program have a language requirement?

Where’s information about mit’s placement record, retention data, etc., i uploaded a transcript to the application system. do i have to mail an official transcript.

No, only admitted applicants are required to mail official transcripts.

Can students take classes at Harvard?

Yes. There is also an annual Harvard/MIT graduate philosophy conference. Often Harvard students attend MIT reading groups and vice versa.

Can philosophy Ph.D. students take linguistics classes?

Yes. We also offer a minor in linguistics.

Can you evaluate my chances of admission before I apply?

We do not offer pre-application screening.

Can I visit MIT before I apply, and talk with faculty and students?

Of course. Send an email to any faculty member , who will be happy to arrange some appointments.

The Department of Linguistics and Philosophy — what’s the story?

Classes in linguistics were originally given within the Department of Modern Languages, and a Ph.D. in linguistics was first offered by that Department in 1961-62. In 1965 the Department of Modern Languages became the Department of Modern Languages and Linguistics and, in 1969, the Department of Foreign Languages and Linguistics. Prior to 1964 philosophy was taught in the Department of Humanities. The Philosophy Department was formed when a Ph.D. program in philosophy was established in July 1971. In 1976 the curriculum in foreign languages and literatures became part of the responsibility of the Department of Humanities while the curriculum in linguistics became part of the Department of Philosophy to form the Department of Linguistics and Philosophy.

  • Graduate Program
  • Undergraduate Program
  • Course Information

Research at MIT Linguistics

Papers and presentations.

UPDATE 7/30: MITWPL is slowly resuming operations. Due to the COVID-19 pandemic and in accordance with MIT’s policies, we have been unable to fulfill orders since mid-March, and as students and non-essential personnel, we are still restricted. We appreciate your continuing patience and understanding, and your support of the MIT graduate student body.

Founded in 1979, MIT Working Papers in Linguistics is an entirely student-managed organization, run by the graduate students in the MIT Doctoral Program in Linguistics. MITWPL is dedicated to promoting student linguistic research by publishing dissertations and papers and, with the proceeds, equitably funding travel, fieldwork and equipment purchases. MITWPL publishes working papers and dissertations for MIT Linguistics and Philosophy , University of Connecticut Linguistics and the University of Rochester Center for Language Sciences . If your department would like to outsource its working paper publications to MITWPL, please contact the management . To browse our extensive list of in-print publications, select ONLINE STORE . If you are an editor of or contributor to an upcoming MITWPL volume, select PUBLISHING . To reach us, click CONTACT .

Recent MITWPL Publications:

50 years of Linguistics at MIT

A scientific reunion, december 9–11, 2011, the development of the linguistics program at the massachusetts institute of technology, 1. introduction.

The linguistics program at MIT evolved in a highly unusual manner for an academic discipline with ancient beginnings. Essentially, the linguistics section was formed not so much to teach a body of knowledge as to contribute to it. From the beginning, research has been primary at MIT. In fact, the linguistics group was originally a research group, associated with the Research Laboratory of Electronics (RLE), with certain of its members also teaching language courses within the Department of Modern Languages. This dual affiliation still exists, with RLE providing the group with various administrative services, a portion of its funding, and physical facilities, but with the majority of the staff holding faculty appointments in what is now the Department of Foreign Literatures and Linguistics.

2. Background: To 1951

In an easily traceable chronology, the origins of the framework within which linguistics was to grow at MIT lie in the development of radar. Even before World War Two, there was increasing interest at the Institute in the new fields of communications and electromagnetic theory, with a concomitant increase in collaboration between physicists and electrical engineers. After the Radiation Laboratory was organized in 1940 to participate in the development of an airborne radar, scientists from still other fields joined the related projects. The impressive output of their combined efforts, both applied and theoretical, encouraged those involved to seek to extend the collaboration into peacetime. Thus, when in 1946 the Radiation Laboratory ceased to exist, a new Research Laboratory of Electronics was formed to absorb its programs. In order to resolve problems of funding and of title to incalculably valuable projects and resources which had been financed by the government, the support of basic research at RLE and other such facilities was given to agencies of the Armed Services. The terms of this sponsorship were such as to allow continuation of the open research climate that had proved so fruitful. [ 1 ]

RLE was a new kind of laboratory. It existed independently of any department at MIT and still does, although laboratory workers are often affiliated with one of the teaching departments as well. The original staff consisted of physicists and electrical engineers, but quite quickly mathematicians, biologists, psychologists, and chemists, as well as linguists and others, were attracted by the communality of interests and the research climate that encouraged it. [ 2 ]

From the start, much of the investigatory effort at RLE was devoted to communications problems of various sorts: in 1946, when the laboratory was formed, one of the four major research groups was labeled “Communications and Related Subjects.” It was probably Norbert Wiener who was most personally responsible for the development of the communication sciences at MIT. His cybernetics, along with Claude Shannon’s mathematical theory of communication, stimulated interested scientists from various fields to probe further and further into the implications of information theory and communications engineering.

In addition to and separate from the RLE communication sciences group there was at MIT an Acoustics Laboratory where work was being carried out on speech and phonetics. Out of this laboratory came the still very active Speech Analysis Group in RLE headed by Kenneth N. Stevens.

An early impetus for the formation of the linguistics group at MIT came from a number of speech communication conferences organized originally by William N. Locke, then Chairman of the Department of Modern Languages. The first meeting on speech analysis, chaired by Locke, was held on 2 December 1949. Marking the opening of formal discussion between linguists and engineers as well as others, the conference was attended by forty scientists, including Roman Jakobson of Harvard, commonly regarded as one of the first linguists to sense the potential of the new scientific developments, Gunnar Fant of the Royal Institute of Technology of Stockholm, who spent some time at MIT and was to coauthor the pioneering Preliminaries to Speech Analysis , and John Lotz of Columbia, who soon afterward gave a series of lectures at MIT on the problems of linguistics.

The interest generated by this meeting led to a more formal Speech Communications Conference, the first of many. It was held at MIT from 31 May to 3 June 1950 under the auspices of the Acoustical Society of America, the Carnegie Project on Scientific Aids to Learning at MIT, and the Psycho-Acoustic Laboratory at Harvard University. An interesting picture of the various approaches to speech communication at the time is afforded by the titles of the papers presented at that meeting: [ 3 ]

As far as the incipient approach to linguistics was concerned, it was clear that interest at this point was directed toward a machine-type analysis of language.

3. The Beginnings: 1951-1955

The character of the speech investigations carried out by Halle and others at RLE in the early fifties was distinctive in its linguistic orientation. Research emphasis was placed on the relationship between the behavior of the human vocal tract and its acoustical output, and the resulting studies provided much information on how the anatomical makeup of man’s vocal tract is utilized in producing the acoustical signal of speech. Examples of acoustical investigation of the period include measurements of the acoustical spectra of consonants, studies of the sounds of Russian, and an attempt to realize a complete automatic recognition scheme for English speech (Hughes (1960)). Speech perception also came under examination, leading to such results as evidence of the influence of context on the perception of stop consonants (Schatz (1954)).

Generally considered to be the most important work to come out of this period, however, because of its influence on the course of phonological study, was Preliminaries to Speech Analysis by Jakobson, Fant and Halle (1952). According to the Preface: (p. v):

This report proposes some questions to be discussed by specialists working on various aspects of speech communication. These questions concern the ultimate discrete components of language, their specific structure, their inventory in the languages of the world, their identification on the acoustical and perceptual levels and their articulatory prerequisites.

Preliminaries was thus an attempt to categorize the phonetic capabilities of the human vocal apparatus by positing a framework underlying the sound structure of all natural languages. Speech sounds are approached as complexes of properties, regarded for a variety of reasons as binary categorial markers. Termed “distinctive features,” these properties are the elements made use of by all languages in the formation of their sound systems.

There was still another area of study which turned out to be a source of stimulation and manpower for the nascent linguistics group, namely, machine translation. Also carried out at RLE as part of the general program of investigation into communication problems but separate from speech research, work on the translation of languages by computer during the 1950’s attracted worldwide attention. The tie-up with linguistics was a direct one: in order to program a machine to translate one language into another, it is necessary to have a thorough grasp of the structure of both languages.

The first conference on machine translation was held at MIT in June 1952. At RLE Y. Bar-Hillel, for one, worked in this area. Also interested was William Locke, who later coedited a book on Machine Translation of Languages (Locke and Booth (1955)). Then, in 1953, Victor Yngve, a physicist who had concerned himself with the idea of translating languages by machine as early as 1949, arrived at MIT. He became director of the Mechanical Translation Research Project at RLE and, with Locke, edited the journal Machine Translation .

Although the course of the future at MIT lay in what was beginning to happen in machine translation and speech analysis research at RLE, there were also, beginning in 1950, two undergraduate linguistics courses. These were taught by Frederick Bodmer, author of The Loom of Language (1944) and a lecturer in the Department of Modern Languages until 1955. The 1950-1951 MIT catalog describes the courses as follows:

L71, L72. SOCIAL LINGUISTICS. Seminar to examine the social functions of language, the interrelations of language and culture; types of languages, primitive and modern; advantages and disadvantages of various systems of writing; the implications of illiteracy; international communication; language planning.

L73, L74. APPLIED SEMANTICS. Seminar to stimulate meaning-consciousness through the discussion of questions such as: What happens when we make or interpret a statement? Why do words get confused with things? What causes semantic blockages? How can they be removed? What are the verbal means of mobilizing emotions? How is language used by the politicians, the salesmen, etc.? What are the most commonly practiced tricks in verbal argument? When is clarity not enough?

In 1952, courses L71, 72 became L75, LANGUAGE AND SOCIETY. The course descriptions remained the same in both cases until 1955.

In their first two years the linguistics courses attracted about twenty students in all. But by 1952 the combined enrollment rose to about forty and remained there until 1961, by which time the offerings had been expanded somewhat.

Courses arising directly out of the communication research at RLE were also beginning to be offered in various departments. The MIT catalog for 1952-1953 lists the following psychology course taught by G. A. Miller:

14.77. PSYCHOLOGY OF COMMUNICATION. Survey of phonetic, acoustic, perceptual, statistical, developmental, behavioral, and social studies of the psychology of language and communication. Particular emphasis is placed upon the effects of verbal habits upon perception, learning, and thinking.

Then, in 1953, Morris Halle and Walter Rosenblith, a specialist in communications biophysics, began to teach a graduate course offered jointly by the Electrical Engineering Department and the Modern Language Department:

6.696. HEARING, SPEECH AND LANGUAGE. Discussion of properties of signals in audio communication systems in the light of characteristics of listeners and speakers. Auditory thresholds, masking, perception of complex acoustic stimuli. Production of natural and synthetic speech. Physical methods of speech analysis. Effects of filtering and clipping. Linguistic units of analysis. Statistical properties of speech signals. Intelligibility criteria for audio communication systems. Integration of experimental data on hearing and speech with recent developments in linguistics and information theory.

The direct descendants of these courses continue to be given by the corresponding departments at MIT today.

To summarize, by 1955 the situation at MIT is as follows. Morris Halle, William Locke, and Victor Yngve are on the faculty of the Modern Language Department and, at the same time, on the staff of RLE, where, as part of the group investigating various aspects of communication, they and others are working on speech analysis (Halle) and machine translation (Yngve). The general tone of the research is to bring to bear upon the study of language the results of the wartime advances in fields such as information theory, communications engineering, and acoustics, while at the same time making available to interested specialists in other areas the results of language study. This was directly reflected in the coursework that came out of RLE research, namely, Halle and Rosenblith’s HEARING, SPEECH, AND LANGUAGE, and Miller’s PSYCHOLOGY OF COMMUNICATION. Also being offered were two undergraduate courses in linguistics, taught by Bodmer. Finally, the most significant published work of the pre-1955 years was Preliminaries to Speech Analysis (Jakobson, Fant, and Halle (1952)), which was to critically affect the nature of phonological investigation for a great many linguists.

4. 1955-1960

Until 1955 the focus of linguistic study at MIT was on the sound structure of language, that is, phonology and morphology, with special emphasis on applied aspects in the form of speech analysis and machine translation. With the arrival of Noam Chomsky that year, however, more and more attention was to be devoted to syntax and to general linguistic theory.

a unified approach to syntactic theory as a whole, including a theory of relations among sentences as an integral part. This latter subtheory… turns out to play a central role in the establishment of the fundamental units of syntax, i.e., in the procedures of constituent analysis… The central conclusion is that a new level of transformational analysis is needed…

Furthermore,

one cannot describe a linguistic system in any meaningful way without some conception of what is the nature of such a system, and what are the properties and purposes of a grammatical description. For this reason it is important to develop a precisely formulated and conceptually complete construction of linguistic theory, based on the clearest possible elementary notions… (Chomsky (1955, pp. 1-3)).

That is, he posited a theory of transformations which was conceived as part of a theory of grammar, “part of an attempt to construct a formalized general theory of linguistic structure and to explore the foundations of such a theory” (Chomsky (1957, p. 5)).

It could probably be agreed that in the two decades since Chomsky’s first publications, his work has stimulated a re-evaluation and ultimately a restatement of the fundamental problem of linguistics. First to feel the impact, quite naturally, was MIT. There the focus of attention began to shift away from the use of linguistic knowledge and methods in the solution of engineering problems. Instead, linguistic theory became the prime concern, and the complexities of syntax attracted more attention as their susceptibility to study appeared to increase.

The evolving theory began to be presented in a number of published works and at various professional meetings, and although there was no formal linguistics program until 1961, interested scholars began to come to MIT. The machine translation project, especially, aided by a Carnegie grant, was able to support the work of a considerable number of linguists during this period, including, among many others, not only Chomsky but Applegate, Matthews, Klima, Lees, Lukoff, and Viertel.

In 1956 Joseph Applegate, who like Chomsky had a Ph.D. in linguistics from the University of Pennsylvania, joined RLE and the Modern Languages Department where he was to relate ongoing work to the study of Berber languages. That same year G. Hubert Matthews, a Ph.D. from the University of Pennsylvania, arrived at RLE and in 1961 became part of the Modern Language faculty as well. With a background in mathematics, German literature, linguistics, and anthropology, he was to become particularly interested in formal languages and in studying American Indian languages in the light of new theoretical devices. Edward Klima, a Ph.D. candidate in linguistics at Harvard, joined the Modern Languages Department in 1957 and the following year became associated with the machine translation group as well. His interests were to center around comparative and historical linguistics, English, and later the study of language acquisition.

In 1959 Jerry Fodor became an instructor in the Department of Humanities. After then spending some time in 1960, along with Jerrold Katz, as a research assistant on the Transformation and Discourse Analysis Project at the University of Pennsylvania, he joined the staff of RLE in 1961. Since that time he has done much work in the philosophy of language, in language perception and acquisition, and with Katz, who also came to RLE in 1961, in the development of a semantic theory. In 1960 Roman Jakobson was officially appointed an Institute Professor in the Department of Modern Languages while still retaining his affiliation with Harvard. It should be pointed out, however, that Jakobson had not only been a Visiting Professor in 1957 but had, from the beginning of Morris Halle’s affiliation with MIT, been deeply interested in the direction of research there. Thus he maintained a close and continuous association with the MIT group long before his official appointment.

The published studies that came out of MIT during the late fifties represented the beginnings of what some have called a new “paradigm” of work in linguistics. Chomsky’s Syntactic Structures in 1957 was preceded not only by the formidable unpublished work on which it was based, i.e., Chomsky (1955), but also by Chomsky, Halle, and Lukoff’s “On Accent and Juncture in English” (1956), where it was shown that intricate stress patterns could be predicted from syntactic structure, and by Chomsky’s “Three Models for the Description of Language” (1956). In 1959 “On Certain Formal Properties of Grammars” appeared, and that same year (1959) saw the publication of what struck many as an explosive review by Chomsky of Skinner’s 1957 work, Verbal Behavior .

Although a good portion of these studies, by virtue of their mathematical approach, seemed inaccessible to many linguists, the theories they represented could not long go unnoticed in a small field where ordinarily everyone read everything and knew everyone. Of great help was the more reachable review of Syntactic Structures in Language (1957) written by Robert Lees, a member of the machine translation project and later a student of Chomsky and Halle’s. Then in 1958 the third of a series of conferences held in Texas on the subject of Problems of Linguistic Analysis in English provided an opportunity for Chomsky to personally present, explicate, and defend his work. Out of this conference came the well-known article on “A Transformational Approach to Syntax” (1962). In 1958 five representatives of MIT presented papers at the annual meeting of the Linguistic Society of America – Applegate, Halle, Lees, Matthews, and Lenneberg (a psychologist doing work related to the MIT approach to language). The following year, 1959, Halle’s Sound Pattern of Russian was published. And in 1960 Halle, Jakobson, and Chomsky were on the program committee of a symposium held in New York on the Structure of Language and its Mathematical Aspects. Chomsky’s “On the Notion ‘Rule of Grammar'” (1961) was presented at this meeting, as was Halle’s paper “On the Role of Simplicity in Linguistic Descriptions” (1961).

By 1960, then, the “MIT school,” as it had come to be known, had undoubtedly emerged as a force to be reckoned with in linguistics, though whether for better or worse was still very much an open question for a good many workers in the field.

An understanding of the early history of MIT linguistics involves explicit recognition of the fact that it was not developed in an intellectual or academic void. By itself it could be interpreted as a synthesis of the seventeenth- and eighteenth-century interest in universal and philosophical grammar and the scientific advances of a new age. However, between the classical period and the late fifties linguistics had taken on a different form, and it was the existing science that the new theories and theorists had to react to. The profoundly different approach that transformationalists espoused was offered to a well-established field, one that had attained a highly refined stage in many areas. In some respects the new approach was seen as growing out of the accomplishments of structural linguistics, particularly with regard to the emphasis on precision of formulation. But in still other, deeper ways it was regarded as a reaction against what were judged to be serious shortcomings in the linguistic science of the fifties. Inevitably, then, a substantial part of the work of this period dealt not only with the revised theoretical and methodological principles themselves, but also with what were interpreted, on the basis of these principles, as weaknesses in modern structuralism. As a result a portion of the writing and speaking took on a certain polemic tone that was to have some effect on work in the field for several years.

Returning now to the increasing exposure MIT ideas were receiving by the end of the fifties, we may note the accompanying increase in interest in them, particularly on the part of scholars attracted to the math/engineering and language/linguistics combination. By 1959 two Ph.D. theses were being completed under the auspices of the Electrical Engineering Department by students working with Chomsky and Halle: one dealt with speech analysis (Hughes (1960)) and the other was Lees’s well-known syntactic work, published the following year (1960) as The Grammar of English Nominalizations . The years 1959 and 1960 saw, in addition, two undergraduate linguistics theses and a Master’s thesis in speech analysis, again carried out under the supervision of Chomsky and/or Halle. Also studying with them on one basis or another were a linguistics student from Columbia, a speech student from Boston University, and a Slavic specialist from Harvard. Still another group of students pursuing academic degrees at different levels and with different specialties were associated with the machine translation project. They included linguists, logicians, mathematicians, and engineers, and they were affiliated with, for example, Harvard, UCLA, Columbia, the University of Texas, Brown, and MIT.

During the years under examination, the actual course offerings in linguistics were somewhat extended. Such expansion was limited, however, by the absence of a degree-granting program, as well as by the research-centered orientation of a still new subject area that was just developing teachable subject matter.

The first change evident in the catalog was in the course description of one of the original undergraduate linguistics courses. In 1955, the year Chomsky took over these courses, the catalog remains unchanged with respect to L75, LANGUAGE AND SOCIETY, although the actual course content undoubtedly underwent some modification. The other course, however, formerly APPLIED SEMANTICS, appears with a new title, a new number, and a new description:

L78. LINGUISTIC STRUCTURE. Problems and methods of the descriptive study of language. Study of form in language : definition and determination of linguistic elements; techniques of analysis; mathematical models for linguistic behavior (communication theory, logical syntax); construction of an objective and abstract theory of formal linguistic structure; systematic application of this theory to the description of English structure. Study of meaning in language : relation of linguistic form and meaning; various approaches to a theory of meaning; contemporary philosophies of language and the influence of symbolic logic; extensional and intensional structure. Comparison of the logic of natural languages with that of special purpose language systems (mathematics, science).

The next change appears in the 1957-1958 catalog, which lists a new undergraduate course in phonology taught by Morris Halle:

L76. PROBLEMS OF PHONOLOGY. Aims and principles of a scientific description of the structure of a spoken language. The mechanism and the acoustics of speech. The phonemic system of a language. The pair test. Distinctive features as a descriptive framework. The phoneme. The interrelation between phonology and other linguistic levels. Some problems of historical phonology. Practical exercises in preparing a phonemic analysis of a language with the help of a native informant.

In 1959-1960 the catalog description of Chomsky’s course on LANGUAGE AND SOCIETY was modified, and the following year Halle took over the teaching of this introductory course:

L75. LANGUAGE AND SOCIETY. Introduction to the scientific study of human language. Outline of syntactic theory; brief survey of English syntax. Elementary data on production and physical nature of speech. The phoneme and distinctive features. Phonemic systems. Morphology: review of some morphological devices. Languages of the world, their historical relationships and structural classification. Speech and writing. Language, culture, and cognition.

Included in the same catalog were two new listings which marked the beginning at MIT of official credit for participation in guided research in linguistics:

L71. SPECIAL PROBLEMS IN LANGUAGE AND LITERATURE.

L72. SPECIAL PROBLEMS IN LANGUAGE AND LITERATURE. Advanced work in literature or linguistics for unusually well-prepared students. Offered by members of staff in their special fields.

In the following year, 1960-1961, a graduate course taught by Yngve was added to cover mathematical aspects:

L79. LANGUAGE, SYMBOLIC PROCESSES, AND COMPUTERS. Symbols, their storage and manipulation: formal systems of symbols, natural languages, computer programming languages, codes, notation; systems of mathematics, logic, physics, etc.; relation of symbols and symbol systems to the physical means of representation and manipulation; relation of symbols and symbol systems to the subject matter represented and its systematization; symbolic processes, induction, deduction, reasoning; translation and translatability.

To summarize, by 1960 the MIT “school” of linguistics had gained a considerable reputation. Interest on the part of graduate students at the Institute and elsewhere was growing, and a few new courses were set up to teach the new subject matter. Involved in MIT research projects were from eight to twelve graduate students from other universities, and additional requests for graduate study in linguistics were being received at a growing rate.

Not surprisingly, then, a proposal for a graduate program in linguistics at MIT was prepared by a committee consisting of Roger Brown (a social psychologist with an interest in the psychology of language), Noam Chomsky, Morris Halle, Roman Jakobson, William Locke, Walter Rosenblith, Jerome Wiesner (now President of MIT and at that time Director of RLE and a leader in the development of communication sciences), and Victor Yngve. The completed proposal was submitted by the Department of Modern Languages on 1 July 1960. The recommendation made was that a graduate program in linguistics be introduced at the Institute in the fall of 1961, under the Department of Modern Languages, leading to the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics. A preference was expressed for at first accepting only candidates who had already proven their competence in one of the two main areas with which the group was concerned, language-linguistics or mathematics-logic-computers.

Our plan is to emphasize the areas in which we have strength and in which training is badly needed by young linguists all over the world. It is not the intent of the Department to staff what might be called a “classical” linguistics department. Thus, we do not expect to have subjects in many of the conventional areas such as the history of particular languages and their development or methods of field work. Students would be expected to master these on their own, or to take subjects in other institutions, such as summer institutes of linguistics.

While linguistics will form the core of our curriculum, we are in a particularly favorable position to capitalize on developments in neighboring fields. The interdepartmental tradition of the Institute makes it possible for linguistics to thrive here in a new and fruitful way. Active interest in the complex problems of language has been shown by members of several departments, and the work in linguistics as outlined here is an essential part of the program of the Center of Communication Sciences. Much work in acoustics, communication sciences, logic, mathematics, neurology, physiology, psychiatry, and psychology is pertinent to linguistics. We hope that some of our students, as well as students from the related fields, will wish to do serious work in the border areas…

(“Graduate Work in Linguistics at the Institute: A Proposal from the Department of Modern Languages” (1960)).

The program was visualized as beginning modestly, with a gradual increase over the first three years to about fifteen students. It was hoped that needed fellowships would come in large part from the National Science Foundation, which had been supporting the research programs in linguistics, and from the National Institutes of Health.

The subjects with immediate application to linguistics being offered at the time were:

6.696. HEARING, SPEECH, AND LANGUAGE. (Graduate) (Described previously.) Rosenblith, Halle

21.76. SYMBOLIC LOGIC. Introduction to the principles of deductive logic. The major results concerning the structure of mathematics. The place of modern logic in philosophy; its influence on the formulation of philosophical problems and on the search for methods for their solution. Chomsky

14.77. PSYCHOLOGY OF LANGUAGE AND COMMUNICATION. Statistical and experimental studies of the psychology of languages. The effects of verbal habits on perception, learning, and communication. Howes

6.574. STATISTICAL THEORY OF INFORMATION. (Graduate) Introduction for graduate students to the quantitative study of communication processes. Definition of a measure of information and study of its properties; representation of messages; efficient coding, transmission in the presence of random disturbances, channel capacity; coding to increase the reliability of transmission. Extension of the theory to continuous channels; signal detection in the presence of disturbances. Fano

L75. LANGUAGE AND SOCIETY. (Described previously.) Halle

L76. PROBLEMS OF PHONOLOGY. (Described previously.) Halle

L78. LINGUISTIC STRUCTURE. (Described previously.) Chomsky

L79. LANGUAGE, SYMBOLIC PROCESSES, AND COMPUTERS. (Described previously.) Yngve

Recommended for addition, ultimately, should the program be approved, were the following subjects:

According to Institute rules and regulations, the authorization of a department to grant a new graduate degree had to come from the MIT Corporation, on the basis of a favorable recommendation from the President on administrative aspects of the proposed program and a recommendation from the MIT Faculty on its academic aspects. The Faculty based its recommendation on the report of the Committee on Graduate School Policy. And this Committee, in turn, generally based its recommendation on the report of a small ad hoc committee set up expressly for the purpose of examining and appraising the academic resources within and available to the department seeking to offer the degree.

The MIT Administration made the decision that the field of linguistics justified support at MIT at a level consistent with its offering the Ph.D. degree. To set the remainder of the necessary machinery in motion, an Ad Hoc Committee on the Linguistics Doctorate was formed to evaluate the adequacy of MIT’s resources for offering the proposed program, and a meeting of the committee was held at MIT on 5 October 1960. The members were:

Professor Patrick Hurley (Chairman of the Committee) Executive Officer Department of Geology and Geophysics MIT

Dr. Bernard Bloch Agassiz Professor of Oriental Languages and Literature Yale University

Dean Peter Elder Graduate School of Arts and Sciences Harvard University

Dr. Einar I. Haugen Visiting Professor Department of German Harvard University and Thompson Professor of Scandinavian Languages University of Wisconsin

Professor Donald Marquis School of Industrial Management MIT

Professor Robert Solow Department of Economics MIT

Also present at the meeting as invited guests were Harold Hazen, Dean of the MIT Graduate School, John Burchard, Dean of Humanities, William Locke, Roman Jakobson, Noam Chomsky, Morris Halle, and Victor Yngve.

The recommendation of the committee was unanimously favorable, and shortly afterward the Graduate Policy Committee met and accepted the report. The following motion was then submitted to and accepted by the meeting of the Faculty:

That the Faculty recommend to the Corporation that it authorize the Department of Modern Languages to recommend the award of the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the field of Linguistics.

The MIT Corporation approved the motion in December 1960.

5. The Degree-Granting Program

5.1. the flavor of the program.

In September 1961 the first officially registered graduate students in linguistics entered MIT. The program they became part of had, it is apparent, a distinctive development. Indeed, it is because the years before its official inception were so critical to the ultimate form and shape of the program that they have been examined so closely here.

The linguistics program developed as a natural outgrowth of the research activities of the MIT linguists, and an attempt is still made to maintain a close tie between teaching and research. Graduate students have always been expected to play an active role in research, that is, to work on real problems, from an early point. The academic program, therefore, was organized so as to encourage students to participate in the ongoing research of the group. This fact should be borne in mind as the different facets of the MIT program are surveyed, for without it the picture is fragmentary. The course descriptions found in the MIT catalog, for example, are only partially revealing. They do not show that the advanced courses particularly, but to some extent the introductory courses as well, take up whatever relevant study the instructor is engaged in at the moment, and that the subject matter of most of the published work to come out of MIT appeared first in one class or another and was shaped by the resulting discussion and criticism. As one of the effects of this classroom approach, there was and still is an unusual amount of “sitting in” on courses by both faculty and students.

A related factor was the absence, during the early years of the program, of an extensive body of immediately relevant literature. Available to the students of the early sixties at MIT were the classical studies and an impressive array of work out of the structuralist school, but very little that dealt directly with transformational generative grammar. The field was simply too new.

Halle and Chomsky deliberately sought to avoid traditionally run courses in which large numbers of students were lectured to on the known works in the field. There was a time, however, when such courses would not have been possible anyway, for there were too few works – known or unknown. Gradually, of course, this changed, but for many years the motto at MIT still remained “If it can be read, it needn’t be taught.” To a certain extent this holds today.

Beginning students, especially those with a traditional academic background, often found the lack of something concrete to study disconcerting. The advice was simply to hang around a lot and let all the talk seep in; eventually it would start to come together. Sometimes this did not happen; more often it did.

Although no particular background is required of program applicants, eligible students in mathematics or science have generally been preferred over those in humanities or social science, all other things being equal. Students who are uncomfortable with scientific constructs and methods may be uncomfortable at MIT, and it is generally more difficult for them to develop a working familiarity with the theory. This is not to say that there is no place for non-science majors at MIT. On the contrary, many are accepted and many do extremely well. Indeed, of the MIT alumni presently on the linguistics faculty, not one had a background in science.

At the beginning the special nature of the MIT program entailed a certain problem for its survival, namely, the finding of qualified staff. A heavy responsibility was thus laid on Halle and Chomsky, and then Klima, in the first few years for the group had to train its own personnel. Klima and Matthews, as we have noted, spent several years at MIT before the program officially began. Paul Postal, with a Ph.D. from the Yale Department of Anthropology, was a research associate for two years before joining the linguistics faculty in 1963. And both Jerrold Katz and Jerry Fodor also put in their RLE years before becoming Assistant Professors on the humanities staff in 1963. Later, Kenneth Hale in 1966 was an exception. But the current faculty includes, in addition to Halle, Chomsky, and Hale, four graduates of the MIT program – Paul Kiparsky, who entered MIT in 1962, and John Ross, James Harris, and David Perlmutter, who became graduate students in 1964.

A further characteristic of study at MIT has been the availability of the faculty for discussion and guidance. It is expected that students will begin serious independent research in the second year of the program, and it is also expected that they will work closely with one or another of the staff as their research progresses. Indeed, it is hoped that they will feel free to discuss any point with any of the instructors at any time: this is necessary if the hang-around-and-let-it-seep-in approach is to work. As the field has grown and pressures on staff members have increased, this approach has been somewhat modified and the easy and open faculty availability of the early days has undergone a certain constriction; nevertheless, there is a constant attempt to preserve as much of it as possible.

It was always understood that to maintain the research emphasis, the informal classroom approach, and the give and take necessary to both, it was imperative to restrict the size of the group. Seven students were accepted the first year, ten the next, ten the year after, and so it went. Since the program is designed to be completed in four years, the entire enrollment of full-time students rarely exceeded thirty-five or so. This number was augmented by post-Doctoral scholars in the program, numbering five or six a year. Today, despite the ascendancy of the MIT school and the expansion of graduate linguistic study in general, limiting the size of the group is still considered necessary to maintain the quality and flavor of the program. Thus, although applicants now total about eighty each year, the number of admissions remains at ten or eleven.

The current program has undergone changes since the early sixties, but by and large they have been changes of degree rather than substance. For example, a substantial literature now exists, and so does a widespread interest in linguistics, even on the undergraduate level. Thus students often arrive at MIT knowing more about transformational generative grammar than advanced theorists knew not so many years ago. At the same time, the enthusiasm that accompanies genuine discovery has passed into the questioning and criticism that arise when a theory has gained wide acceptance and is being pushed to its limits. As a result, although students are better prepared in some ways to deal with what they find at MIT, they are still not offered a Body of Truth to learn.

There are now some classes, more things to read, more people to talk to. But there is probably also more pressure and, as a result of alternative ways of grappling with the theory, more dissension. There are more introductory courses and they are a good deal more introductory than they ever were, but the opportunities for casual faculty-student contact have narrowed. On balance, it could probably be said that certain focuses have shifted and certain extremes have been blunted, but the particular flavor of the MIT linguistics program appears in general to persist.

No survey of this program would be complete without mention of the special role Morris Halle has played in its development. Others have given significantly of their time and their efforts, as well as their brainpower. But it is generally agreed that Halle, intellectually, spiritually, and practically, has been the strongest single force in building the group and in guiding and guarding its growth.

5.2. Official Aspects of the Program

In order to be accepted as a full-time student in the linguistics program, it is necessary to satisfy Institute requirements for admission to the Graduate School and to have done well in previous academic work. However, beyond competence (i.e., three years of formal study) in two languages in addition to English, there are no explicit prerequisites in terms of courses or fields of specialty. Rather, applicants should demonstrate evidence of their ability to effectively engage in serious study of complex subject matter. This may take the form of academic work of very high quality, original research, challenging work experience, in-depth mastery of a language or language group, and so on.

Once a student has been accepted in the linguistics program, he is normally required to take a “core” of linguistics courses consisting of the following (the courses will be described in a later section):

A full-time student is expected to take the equivalent of four subjects for credit each semester.

The degree candidate must also complete a program of studies in a minor field, the choice of which is made by the student, subject to the approval of the department. The minor program ordinarily involves three one-semester graduate subjects, selected so as to form a meaningful whole. Likely fields for the completion of minor studies include electrical engineering, mathematics, philosophy, psychology, anthropology, literary theory, and advanced work in some group of languages. Work done before entering MIT is sometimes accepted as satisfying all or part of the minor requirement. Students may also take advantage of the cross-registration agreement with Harvard to complete their minor.

In the middle of the second year of full-time study, students normally take a general examination given by the linguistics section. Originally this was a written examination testing the following areas – general linguistics (problems of grammatical and phonemic structure), linguistic theories and methods, practical linguistic analysis, the relation between linguistics and adjacent fields, and questions on the student’s special field. Later the last two topics were changed to historical linguistics and the structure of English. Still later (1970), the examination was replaced by the required preparation of two original in-depth studies, one syntactic and the other phonological. A board of three faculty members then administers an oral examination on the papers submitted by the student and related topics. Passage or failure is determined by the linguistics faculty, and revision of one or both of the papers and/or re-examination may be requested.

Finally, there is the research leading to a dissertation project, the completion of the dissertation, and the passing of an oral thesis defense.

From time to time, at the discretion of the department, special students are admitted who wish to study in the program without being degree candidates. Also admitted are post-Doctoral fellows who demonstrate a serious interest in the study of linguistics as undertaken at MIT. Such appointments are normally made for a period of one year.

5.3. Substance of the Program

Although the original proposal by the linguistics group called for the admission of from three to five students each year at first, the program actually started with seven regular students, one of whom later left, and three special or part-time students. The regular students included a psychologist, a chemist, a philosopher, an electrical engineer, and several mathematicians. [ 4 ] All are at present actively involved at various institutions.

The linguistics section of the newly formalized Department of Modern Languages and Linguistics (MIT course of study number 23) began with a staff of six: Jakobson, Halle, Chomsky, Klima, Matthews, and Yngve (who by then was officially affiliated with the Department of Electrical Engineering but maintained his contact with the linguistics group). Furthermore, a significant shift was taking place: both interest and funding were now settling on the linguistics program itself, and researchers who were becoming increasingly concerned with linguistics in its theoretical aspects no longer had to operate through machine translation or other projects. It now became possible for the linguistics group directly to support the work of interested scholars such as S. Jay Keyser, Carlotta Smith, and a good many others.

Eleven linguistics courses were offered in the first year of the program, five of which (marked with asterisks) were new:

*23.701. CRUCIAL PROBLEMS IN LINGUISTICS. Word: form and meaning, grammatical and lexical analysis. Word classes and morphological categories. … Jakobson

23.71. SPECIAL PROBLEMS IN LANGUAGE AND LITERATURE.

23.72. SPECIAL PROBLEMS IN LANGUAGE AND LITERATURE. Advanced work in literature or linguistics for unusually well-prepared students. Offered by members of staff in their special fields.

*23.731. STRUCTURE OF ENGLISH. Syntax : detailed study of the grammatical structure of sentences in contemporary English. Special reference to formal correlates of traditional grammatical statements about English syntax, such as those by Jespersen, Poutsma, and Curme. Consideration of current descriptions of English syntax, such as those by Harris, Hill, and Fries. Klima

*23.732. STRUCTURE OF ENGLISH. Morphology and phonology . Continuation of 23.731. The interrelation of syntactic, morphological, and phonological phenomena in the English language. The phonological system and the structure of morphemes. Special reference to current interpretations by leading structuralists. The dialectology and historical phonology of English. Klima

23.751 LANGUAGE AND SOCIETY. Introduction to the scientific study of human language. Outline of syntactic theory; brief survey of English syntax. Elementary data on production and physical nature of speech. The phoneme and distinctive features. Phonemic systems. Morphology: review of some morphological devices. Languages of the world, their historical relationships and structural classification. Speech and writing. Language, culture, and cognition. Halle

23.761. PROBLEMS OF PHONOLOGY. Aims and principles of a scientific description of the structure of a spoken language. The mechanism and the acoustics of speech. The phonemic system of a language. The pair test. Distinctive features as a descriptive framework. The phoneme. The interrelation between phonology and other linguistic levels. Some problems of historical phonology. Practical exercises in preparing a phonemic analysis of a language with the help of a native informant. Halle

*23.781. SURVEY OF GENERAL LINGUISTICS. Critical study of a series of major works on the nature of language. Readings in linguistics, philosophy, and psychology. Chomsky

23.782. LINGUISTIC STRUCTURE. Problems and methods of the descriptive study of language. Study of form in language: definition and determination of linguistic elements; mathematical models for linguistic behavior; construction of an objective and abstract theory of formal linguistic structure; systematic application of this theory to the description of English structure. Study of meaning in language: relation of linguistic form and meaning; various approaches to a theory of meaning; contemporary philosophy of language and the influence of symbolic logic; extensional and intensional structure. Comparison of the logic of natural languages with that of special purpose language systems (i.e., mathematics, science). Chomsky

23.791 LANGUAGE, SYMBOLIC PROCESSES, AND COMPUTERS. Symbols, their storage and manipulation: formal systems of symbols, natural languages, computer programming languages, codes, notation systems of mathematics, logic, physics, etc.; relation of symbols and symbol systems to the physical means of representation and manipulation; relation of symbols and symbol systems to the subject matter represented and its systematization; symbolic processes, induction, deduction, reasoning; translation and translatability. Yngve

*23.729. MECHANICAL TRANSLATION AND LANGUAGE PROCESSING. A survey of research in the area of language processing by machine. Successes and inadequacies of current techniques. Remaining obstacles to adequate mechanical translation. Morphology; syntax; lexicography; semantics; automatic analysis and synthesis of sentences. Structure of English, German, and other languages. Yngve

These courses were, as we have noted, supplemented by a number of related courses in other departments. One change that took place was in the graduate course offered by Electrical Engineering and taught by Halle and Rosenblith since 1953. It became an undergraduate course handled by Halle and Kenneth N. Stevens, but it was still available to interested linguistics students:

6.36 SPEECH COMMUNICATION. For well-qualified students with a serious interest in speech communication. Survey of structural properties of natural languages with special emphasis on the sound pattern. Acoustical theory of speech production; anatomical considerations; the vocal tract as a generator of acoustical signals; acoustical correlates of vocal-tract configurations, both stationary and dynamic. Perception of speech: the auditory capabilities of man; the role of set in perception; analysis by synthesis. Mechanical recognition and generation of speech. Speech transmission and compression: the concept of intelligibility; effects of distortion on intelligibility. Stevens, Halle

It should be noted that the official beginning of the graduate linguistics program marked the official end of undergraduate linguistics courses at MIT for almost a decade. Although qualified undergraduates could and did enroll in several of the courses, there was not to be another specifically designed undergraduate introductory course until 1970.

Reviewing the first-year courses, it is immediately evident that the linguistics offerings had been expanded but not altered in any radical way. The aim and the scope of the teaching program remained entirely consistent with those of the research program. The mathematical-computer and machine translation aspects of the field were covered, as was the other applied form, speech analysis; but there was a greater number of courses relating to theoretical aspects. English was the only language covered in any comprehensive way, and it was treated in fairly great depth and breadth. In short, as the original proposal had stated, there was no attempt to duplicate linguistics programs elsewhere.

It might be noted that although this was undoubtedly deliberate, it might not have been possible without the presence of Harvard some blocks away, together with a long-standing exchange program that permitted full-time graduate students from one university to take as much as half their work at the other. [ 5 ]

The linguistics group remained subsumed under the retitled Department of Modern Languages and Linguistics, which in 1969 was renamed the Department of Foreign Literatures and Linguistics. Its ties to the Research Laboratory of Electronics were also maintained, and in 1961 there were a number of related projects being carried out there.

Ten new graduate students were admitted in the fall of 1962, the second year of the program’s existence, bringing to sixteen the total enrolled for a degree. This time there were linguists in the group as well as mathematicians, a computer specialist, a biologist, and a philosopher-psychologist. One failed to complete the program and a second went into medicine after finishing; the rest remain active in the field.

There were a number of staff additions and new courses that year. The noted Indo-Europeanist Jerzy Kuryłowicz of the University of Krakow accepted an appointment as Visiting Professor. G. Hubert Matthews, who had been teaching German, initiated a course in TYPOLOGY OF GRAMMARS:

23.783 TYPOLOGY OF GRAMMARS. Study of the grammars of languages belonging to divergent families with a view to reaching a sharper characterization of universal features of grammatical structure, with the majority of examples to be drawn from non-Indo-European languages.

Barbara Hall (Partee), then a second-year graduate student in the program, began to teach a new course designed to convey to non-science students the mathematical concepts and tools needed to work with linguistics at MIT:

23.771. MATHEMATICAL BACKGROUNDS FOR COMMUNICATION SCIENCES. Fundamentals of discrete mathematics for non-mathematicians. Introduction to various topics in set theory, probability and information theory, logic, foundations, theory of automata and computability.

There were four additional new courses:

23.702. SEMINAR IN LINGUISTICS. Special problems in linguistics. Permission of instructor required.

23.734. STRUCTURE OF RUSSIAN. Survey of the syntax, morphology, and phonology of contemporary literary Russian, with emphasis on the application of modern theory to classical problems of Slavic linguistics. Consideration of the relevant dialectological and historical evidence. (Adequate knowledge of Russian required.) Halle, Klima

23.772. MATHEMATICAL MODELS IN LINGUISTICS. Study of mathematical models that relate to linguistic competence and performance. Formal properties of grammatical systems. Grammars of natural and artificial languages. Models of the speaker and hearer that incorporate representations of his knowledge of the language. Probabilistic models for the user. Stress on inter-connections of theory of grammar and theory of automata, and implications for the study of human cognitive processes wherever possible. Chomsky

23.785. LINGUISTIC CHANGE. Review of instances of change in phonology, morphology, and syntax. Examples drawn primarily from histories of Indo-European languages with special emphasis on English and Russian. An attempt to develop a general theory of linguistic change. Halle, Klima

Chomsky took over the teaching of the first semester of STRUCTURE OF ENGLISH to free Klima to participate in the above new courses. The description of Jakobson’s course was modified:

23.701. CRUCIAL PROBLEMS IN LINGUISTICS. Sound and meaning: a study of the various types of relationships between phonetic and semantic aspects of language.

(The last two courses listed were offered jointly by Modern Languages and Electrical Engineering.)

An important occurrence for the MIT linguistics group in 1962 was the meeting there of the Ninth International Congress of Linguists. (Chomsky’s Current Issues in Linguistic Theory (1964) was presented at that Congress in an early form.)

The year 1963 again saw the admission of ten new students, with what was becoming the typical diversity of backgrounds. Accordingly there were further additions to the staff and to the roster of courses. Matthews, formerly a research associate, became an Associate Professor, and Paul Postal, who had been associated with RLE, became an Assistant Professor in the department. Moreover, Jerrold Katz and Jerry Fodor, who had also been at RLE, joined the staff of the Humanities Department.

The addition of Katz and Fodor to the MIT teaching staff made possible two undergraduate courses which, though given by the Humanities Department, were directly relevant to linguistics:

21.87. PHILOSOPHY OF LANGUAGE. Current philosophical approaches to questions about language in general and meaning in particular. Study of authors representative of the Positivist movement and of Ordinary Language Philosophy. Particular attention to such topics as the “performative” analysis of speech and the implications of the philosophy of language for philosophical psychology. Fodor

21.88. SEMANTICS. Characterization of the form of a semantic theory of a natural language. Basic consideration of (1) phenomena falling within the domain of a semantic theory and (2) descriptive goals of a semantic theory, the kind of theory employed in pursuit of these goals, and the empirical and methodological requirements upon an adequate semantic theory. Special attention to the role of a semantic theory in the solution of philosophical problems. (Additional requirement: thorough knowledge of material in Chomsky’s Syntactic Structures .) Fodor, Katz

Added directly to the linguistics offerings were the following two courses:

23.703. LINGUISTICS AND POETICS. Prosodic patterns in word-and-sentence phonology; their utilization in metrics; prosodic and metrical typology. Metrical role of inherent distinctive features. Morphological and syntactic constituents of verse. Jakobson

23.736. STRUCTURE OF GERMAN. Survey of the syntax, morphology, and phonology of contemporary literary German, with emphasis on the application of modern theory to classical problems of Germanic linguistics. Consideration of the relevant historical evidence. (Adequate knowledge of German required.) Klima

And the descriptions of STRUCTURE OF ENGLISH and LINGUISTIC STRUCTURE were reworded as follows:

23.731. STRUCTURE OF ENGLISH. Introduction to the grammatical devices of English. The interrelation of syntactic, morphological, and phonological phenomena in the English language. The phonological system and the structure of phonemes. Special reference to current interpretations by leading structuralists. Chomsky

23.732. STRUCTURE OF ENGLISH. Detailed study of the grammatical structure of sentences in contemporary English. Special reference to formal correlates of traditional grammatical statements about English syntax, such as those by Jespersen, Poutsma, and Curme. Klima

23.752. LINGUISTIC STRUCTURE. The empirical requirements for a theory of linguistic structure that will encompass and account for linguistic competence and the ability to use and acquire it. Study of aspects of English syntax, semantics, and phonology insofar as they furnish critical tests for such a theory. Systematic presentation of a theory of linguistic structure, its abstract properties and range of empirical adequacy. Explanatory models in linguistics; their implications concerning the nature of cognitive processes. Chomsky

It is clear that at the close of the academic year 1963-1964, with twenty-six students, six of whom had completed three years and were deeply into their dissertations, with a growing number of special students and post-doctoral scholars, with a staff of six, plus three members of other departments, offering a roster of more than twenty courses, and with RLE research activities continuing, MIT had a full-scale linguistics program in operation.

In applying for an NIH training grant in 1964, the group described the ultimate questions toward which their research was directed in the following terms: [ 6 ]

What do the artifacts of communication and in particular language tell us about the natural capabilities of man?; or what inferences can be made concerning the intrinsic capabilities of the organism that masters and utilizes the systems of expression and communication? …

The research strategy for seeking answers to these questions was then reviewed:

For each language we attempt to construct abstractly a device (called technically a generative grammar of this language), which specifies the structural information about sentences that is available in principle to the speakers of the language. … A generative grammar provides an explicit account of one primary aspect of the state of the perceiving organism… Thus language provides a unique opportunity for precise formulation and fruitful empirical study of some of the central questions of perceptual psychology.

…our training and research programs have a strongly interdisciplinary flavor, and consequently most of the faculty have close contacts with members of other departments and are frequently involved in joint research projects with members of other departments at MIT and Harvard. Most of our students have similar interests, or acquire them during their graduate training, and we attempt to encourage their participation in such work…as fully as possible.

The major research interests of the staff were sketched as follows:

Theory of Grammar (Chomsky, Jakobson, Katz, Postal) The primary goal of linguistic investigation is to construct grammars for particular languages and, on the basis of these, to determine the general properties of any human language. A grammar represents in a precise way the ability of the speaker to understand an arbitrary sentence of his language and to produce sentences (in general, new sentences) on the appropriate occasion. Thus a grammar pairs phonetically represented signals with semantic interpretations; it generates an infinite set of such pairs. … The theory of grammar is concerned with the overall structure of grammar, the interrelations of these subdomains (syntax, phonology, and semantics), the question of evaluating grammars and determining their empirical adequacy. The problem of evaluating grammars, which is central to the theory of grammar, is essentially a fundamental aspect of the problem of accounting for the ability of the child to learn a language. …

Formal Languages (Chomsky, Halle, Matthews) When the properties of a generative grammar are formulated with sufficient precision, they can be studied abstractly, apart from any question of empirical interpretation. This purely mathematical study of grammars…has been quite fruitful. For one thing, it has been shown that some simple types of grammars relate very closely to systems that have an independent, automata-theoretic interest, as well as to familiar concepts of logic and even classical mathematics that on the surface appear quite independent. Furthermore, properties of these formal systems…have on several occasions suggested empirical consequences concerning actual languages. Ultimately, one hopes to be able to study the mathematical structure of generative systems of a type that are adequate for natural language. …

Syntax (Chomsky, Klima, Matthews, Postal) The syntactic component of a grammar generates an infinite set of structures which support phonological and semantic interpretation. … Our work in syntax divides naturally into two subparts, theoretical and applied. In theoretical syntax we are concerned to formulate the general properties of this recursive system…in as explicit and narrow a fashion as possible, on the basis of the data concerning syntactic structures of given languages which is provided by the work in applied syntax.

Phonology (Chomsky, Halle, Jakobson, Matthews, Postal) The phonological component of a grammar is an input-output system which converts surface structures, provided by the syntactic component, into phonetic representations. The problems of phonology are to determine the character of the rules that constitute this system and the manner of their functioning, and to determine the elementary units (the universal system of phonetic distinctive features) and general laws regarding these units that delimit the set of possible phonetic representations. This work is naturally heavily dependent on detailed phonological and phonetic investigation of particular languages. A particularly important aspect of current work concerns the syntactic determinants of phonetic structure. …

Semantics (Fodor, Katz, Postal) The semantic component of a grammar assigns semantic interpretations to the deep structures provided by the syntactic component. Research into the structure of the semantic component is quite recent, but highly promising. … Thus, the theory of semantics is concerned to discover a general system of semantic representation…and to determine the nature of the “projection rules” that assign semantic interpretations to larger units on the basis of the semantic interpretations of their parts. …

Historical Linguistics (Halle, Jakobson, Klima, Matthews) Historical linguistics is the name given to the study of the changes that grammars of languages undergo in the course of time. Since grammars are ordered sets of rules, historical linguistics is primarily interested in the changes that affect these rules. It seems that the most important mechanism of linguistic change is the addition of a single rule to the grammar. … As different rules are added to different dialects the dialects will differ more and more. … A surprising result of our studies appears to be that…radical differences are not found; instead, languages conserve their grammars intact over many generations…and change only slowly as the number of rules added increases. Instances where radical differences between the grammars of two generations can be established are rare. …

The linguistic theories being studied in this program strongly suggest that the process of language learning involves a theory-building mechanism available to human infants and depends in only a minor manner on repetitious and rote reinforcement. …

Models of Visual Perception (Eden, [ 8 ] Chomsky, Fodor) A consistent interpretation of the orientation and depth judgments in optical illusions, as well as a description of human performance in the psychophysics of space, can be made by constructing an abstract (generative) theory for the geometry of the visual perceptual world. A visual impression is “recognized” when the postulates adopted by the observer generate a precept well-formed according to the rules of the theory.

Handwriting (Eden, Halle) In a manner analogous to the techniques of phonology, cursive writing in a given script can be characterized by a finite sequence of discrete symbols (strokes). These strokes may be thought to represent sub-routines or algorithms for generating an appropriate segment of the written text. Such descriptions have been successfully applied to several styles of writing. Moreover, the technique has been extended so as to program a device to “read” handwritten English words. …

Linguistics applicants in 1964 totaled over forty; eleven were selected to become part of the program. Permanent-staff expansion came to a temporary halt that year. Aside from the addition of two Visiting Professors – Jerzy Kuryłowicz (for whom this marked a return) and Peter Colaclides – the only change was the replacement of William Locke as Chairman of the Department of Modern Languages and Linguistics by William F. Bottiglia, who taught French and Italian literature in the department. Chomsky was on leave, working at the Center for Cognitive Studies at Harvard, and Postal took over the teaching of STRUCTURE OF ENGLISH I, LINGUISTIC STRUCTURE, and SURVEY I.

The subject offerings, on the other hand, did undergo a number of changes. First, the list of courses was increased by three:

23.754 FIELD METHODS. Survey of means for eliciting linguistically relevant data about a language from a native speaker; for obtaining correct phonetic representations, the basic phonological system, and basic grammatical structure. Suggestions for dealing with informants, elicitation, and data organization. Matthews, Postal

23.786. SEMINAR ON LINGUISTIC CHANGE. Continuation of 23.785, LINGUISTIC CHANGE. Halle, Klima, Matthews

Also, the description of Jakobson’s course was revised as follows:

23.701. CRUCIAL PROBLEMS IN LINGUISTICS. The history and present state of phonemic analysis. Phonemes and distinctive features. Phonetic typology and universals.

The catalog furthermore lists the following two relevant courses offered in Psychology, the first of which was an undergraduate course and was at that point taught by Thomas Bever, a linguistics graduate student:

9.59. PSYCHOLOGY OF LANGUAGE AND COMMUNICATION. Psychological theories of meaning and reference. Verification of statistical and descriptive linguistic models by psychological experimentation, study of language development and study of aphasia. Application of linguistic models to animal communication systems and other complexly structured behavior patterns. Bever

9.591. SEMINAR IN PSYCHOLOGY OF LANGUAGE AND COMMUNICATION. Reading, discussion, and criticism of selected topics. One or another of the following themes will be emphasized in a particular semester: statistical theories of language, speech perception, communication in subhuman species, experimental semantics, aphasia, language acquisition, language and thought. Fodor

With all the research that was being carried on, a respectable literature was being built up. The most significant 1964 additions were Postal’s Constituent Structure , Fodor and Katz’s collection of the major relevant articles up to that time, and Katz and Postal’s An Integrated Theory of Linguistic Descriptions , wherein a significant modification of the theory of linguistic descriptions was proposed.

Furthermore, an increasing number of languages were being studied within the framework of the developing theory. In addition to English, Russian, and German, which were continually being subjected to analysis in MIT classes, there was Paul Postal’s work on Mohawk, Matthews’ studies of Siouan languages (e.g., (1964)), and student investigations of Slavic (Lightner), French (Schane), Japanese (McCawley), and Spanish (Foley and then Harris). [ 9 ]

The year 1965 saw the loss to the MIT staff of Paul Postal and the addition of Paul Kiparsky. The latter had come to MIT as a student in 1962 and, with a background in Indo-European and Germanic, was to make important contributions to phonological theory. In his first year of teaching he was responsible for the SURVEY I course as well as portions of FIELD METHODS, LINGUISTIC CHANGE, and the SEMINAR ON LINGUISTIC CHANGE. Katz that year assumed sole responsibility for the related philosophy courses; they became graduate courses and the description of PHILOSOPHY OF LANGUAGE was modified as follows:

21.722T. PHILOSOPHY OF LANGUAGE. Study of recent philosophical movements concerned with the treatment of traditional philosophical problems from a linguistic movement; in particular, logical positivism and ordinary language philosophy. Consideration of the treatment of philosophical problems within the framework of linguistic theory. Some acquaintance with Chomsky’s theory of generative grammar desirable.

This was the year of the dissertation: eleven theses were accepted in 1965, representing the work of students of the first two classes to enter and dealing with various topics in phonology, syntax, and theory. [ 10 ] Published in 1965 was Chomsky’s major study of syntactic theory, Aspects of the Theory of Syntax .

1966-1967, 1967-1968

With the staff additions in these years, the linguistics faculty reached its present composition except for David Perlmutter, who did not join the staff until 1970. John R. Ross, who had come to MIT as a linguistics student in 1964 and who has since made major contributions to the development of syntactic theory, was appointed Assistant Professor, as was James W. Harris, whose area of concentration is Romance phonology with particular emphasis on Spanish and who had also begun as a student in the program in 1964. The group lost the services of Edward S. Klima, who had played a significant role in the program since its formative years, but gained Kenneth Hale, a specialist in the study of non-Indo-European languages.

The Visiting Professors in these years were James Sledd and J. Frits Staal, who were responsible for the following additions to the catalog:

23.733. HISTORY OF THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE. Introduction to the external and internal history of the language and to the history of its study from Old English to the present. A reading knowledge of Old and Middle English desirable. Sledd

23.737. INTRODUCTION TO SANSKRIT. Brief review of writing system, phonology, morphology, and syntax of the language. Staal

23.788. THE PĀNINIAN TRADITION IN LINGUISTICS. Staal

Other new or reorganized courses that appeared were:

23.766. LABORATORY IN THE PHYSIOLOGY, ACOUSTICS, AND PERCEPTION OF SPEECH. Experimental investigations of speech processes. Measurement of pressure and volume velocity during speech. Interpretation of X-ray motion pictures. Computer-aided analysis and display of acoustic waveforms and spectra, spectrographic analysis, acoustic correlates of distinctive features. Problems in automatic speech recognition; techniques of speech synthesis; studies of speech perception. Halle, Klatt

23.702. SEMINAR IN LANGUAGE ACQUISITION. Discussion of the theoretical and factual basis for a model of language learning. Treatment of philosophical, psychological, and linguistic issues concerning language learning as well as the recent evidence from studies on children’s language skills. Klima

23.705. SEMINAR ON POETIC FORM. Study of the organization of linguistic elements in metrical verse. Halle

23.751. INTRODUCTION TO LINGUISTICS I: SYNTAX. Deep and surface structure. Phrase structure and transformational rules. Recursive mechanisms: relative clauses, complements, conjunctions. Types of ordering. Exception mechanisms and pruning. Universality of deep structure. Halle, Ross

23.762. INTRODUCTION TO LINGUISTICS II: PHONOLOGY. Aims and principles of a scientific description of the phonic aspect of language. The mechanism and acoustics of speech. Distinctive features as the descriptive framework. The phoneme. The phonemic system of a language. The interrelations between phonology and other linguistic levels. Some problems of historical phonology. Practical exercises. Halle

23.758. INTRODUCTION TO LINGUISTICS III: THEORY OF GRAMMAR. Elementary transformations and derived constituent structure. Rule government. Variables. Identity. Indexing. The abstract nature of underlying structures. Ross

23.738. SEMINAR ON ENGLISH SYNTAX. Continuation of STRUCTURE OF ENGLISH II. Ross

23.783. TOPICS IN THE GRAMMAR OF NON-INDO-EUROPEAN LANGUAGES. Detailed examination of the grammar of a non-Indo-European language with special emphasis on problems of interest to general linguistics. Hale

23.784. SEMINAR ON UNIVERSAL GRAMMAR. Study of grammatical problems in several languages with a view towards achieving a sharper characterization of certain universal features of grammatical structure. Matthews

Note that Halle and Ross together were now teaching an introductory set of courses.

The description for Jakobson’s course was revised as usual to reflect directly the change in content each year:

23.701. CRUCIAL PROBLEMS IN LINGUISTICS. The Moscow school – Fortunatov, Trubetzkoy, further stages – and its place in world linguistics.

Ross took over the STRUCTURE OF ENGLISH II course, with the resulting change in description:

23.732. STRUCTURE OF ENGLISH II: ADVANCED SYNTAX. Selected topics in the syntax of English.

And Kiparsky taught the course on Indo-European:

Kenneth Hale became responsible for FIELD METHODS and participated in the teaching of LINGUISTIC CHANGE and SEMINAR ON LINGUISTIC CHANGE. Thus by 1967-1968 the roster of linguistics courses was as follows:

There were also, as we have noted, directly relevant courses being taught in other departments, especially Humanities and Psychology. With regard to these, the descriptions of Fodor’s psychology courses were altered:

9.59 PSYCHOLOGY OF LANGUAGE AND COMMUNICATION. Study of psychology of language processes in light of recent advances in structural linguistics; extensive critique of learning-theoretic accounts of verbal behavior; examination of problems of language acquisition and speech perception. Methodological implications of psycholinguistic experiments and theory will be stressed.

9.591. SEMINAR IN PSYCHOLOGY OF LANGUAGE AND COMMUNICATION. Reading, discussion, and criticism of selected topics. One or more of the following themes will be emphasized in a particular semester: theories of language, speech perception, communication in sub-human species, aphasia, language acquisition, language and thought, interaction between grammatical structure and verbal performance.

Undoubtedly the most important study of these years was the long-awaited Sound Pattern of English (1968) by Chomsky and Halle, still the standard work in generative phonological theory and its application. Earlier, on other topics, Chomsky’s Cartesian Linguistics (1966) and Katz’s Philosophy of Language (1966) had appeared.

1968-1969 to 1973-1974

Since 1967 the only permanent addition to the linguistics faculty itself has been David Perlmutter. After having taught Russian at MIT for three years, he joined the linguistics program as a student in 1964 and returned as a staff member in 1970. Perlmutter is able to take advantage of a speaking knowledge of fourteen languages in his work on universal grammar and syntactic theory. Wayne O’Neil, a specialist in English who had been at MIT previously for post-doctoral study, became affiliated with the Humanities Department during this period and began to teach several courses that are offered jointly with the linguistics section. Finally, in 1973 James Harris assumed the chairmanship of the Department of Foreign Literatures and Linguistics. In 1973-1974, then, the linguistics staff was constituted as follows: [ 13 ]

In addition, teaching relevant courses in other departments were:

Course offerings in 1973-1974 are given below, with additions or significant changes since 1967 marked with an asterisk: [ 14 ]

*23.700. LANGUAGE AND ITS STRUCTURE. (Undergraduate) An introduction to basic concepts and issues for the non-linguist. Production and physical properties of speech, phonetic and phonemic representations, deep and surface syntactic structures, language change, methods and goals of modern linguistic theories. Harris

*23.702. SEMINAR ON CURRENT ISSUES IN LINGUISTICS. Examination of selected topics that are of particularly timely interest. Kiparsky, Halle

23.705. SEMINAR ON POETIC FORM. Study of the organization of linguistic elements in metrical verse. Halle, Kiparsky

*23.706. LINGUISTICS AND LITERARY STUDY. Study of the formal properties of the language of literature. Consideration of the insight that linguistic theory gives into the development of a general theory of literature. O’Neil

23.711. SPECIAL PROBLEMS IN LANGUAGE AND LITERATURE

23.712. SPECIAL PROBLEMS IN LANGUAGE AND LITERATURE. Advanced work in literature or linguistics for unusually well-prepared students. Offered by members of staff in their special fields. Staff

23.723. SEMANTICS AND THE PHILOSOPHY OF LANGUAGE. Characterization of a semantic theory within transformational grammar, and a discussion of the philosophical issues. Semantic topics: the goals of a semantic theory, the formal representation of sense, presupposition, predication structure, semantic categories, temporal and converse relations, the controversy between generative and interpretive semantics. Philosophical topics: the analytic-synthetic distinction, the issue between effability and indeterminacy, the nature of logic, referential opacity, and speech acts. Katz

*23.724. ADVANCED SEMANTICS. An investigation of the adequacy of semantic apparatus for representing semantic structures. Emphasis on the most recent proposals about semantic representation. Members of the seminar attempt to construct apparatus in some cases where no proposal has been made. Katz

*23.731. LINGUISTIC STRUCTURES: ROMANCE. Topics in the syntax, phonology, and morphology of the Romance languages, with emphasis on the application of modern theory to classical problems of Romance linguistics and the implications of data drawn from the Romance languages for general linguistic theory. (Some knowledge of a Romance language required.) Hale, Harris, Perlmutter

23.733. HISTORY OF THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE. Selected topics in the history of English syntax, morphology, and phonology from Old English to the present, formulated within an attempt at a general theory of linguistic change. (Some knowledge of Old and Middle English desirable.) O’Neil

23.724. STRUCTURE OF RUSSIAN. Survey of the syntax, morphology, and phonology of contemporary literary Russian, with emphasis on the application of modern theory to classical problems of Slavic linguistics. Consideration of the relevant dialectological and historical evidence. (Adequate knowledge of Russian required.)

23.736. STRUCTURE OF GERMAN. Survey of the syntax, morphology, and phonology of contemporary literary German, with emphasis on the application of modern theory to classical problems of Germanic linguistics. Consideration of the relevant historical evidence. (Adequate knowledge of German required.) Kiparsky, Ross

23.741. TOPICS IN THE GRAMMAR OF A NON-INDO-EUROPEAN LANGUAGE.

23.742. TOPICS IN THE GRAMMAR OF A NON-INDO-EUROPEAN LANGUAGE. Detailed examination of the grammar of a language whose structure is significantly different from English, with special emphasis on problems of interest in the study of linguistic universals. Course assistance by a native speaker of the language, when possible. Hale

23.751 INTRODUCTION TO LINGUISTICS I: SYNTAX. Introduction to the process of formulating and testing hypotheses to account for syntactic data in natural languages. Emphasis on the role of metatheory in choosing between competing hypotheses. Grounding in basic syntactic processes: deep and surface structure, phrase structure and transformational rules, recursive mechanisms, rule ordering, and the transformational cycle. Data are drawn primarily from English. Perlmutter

23.752. INTRODUCTION TO LINGUISTICS III: THEORY OF GRAMMAR. Systematic presentation of a theory of linguistic structure, its abstract properties and range of empirical adequacy. Chomsky

*23.753. ADVANCED GRAMMAR I

*23.754. ADVANCED GRAMMAR II. Selected topics in phonology, syntax, and semantics. Ross, Hale

23.755. LINGUISTIC STRUCTURE. The empirical requirements for a theory of linguistic structure that will encompass and account for linguistic competence and the ability to use and acquire it. Study of aspects of English syntax, semantics, and phonology insofar as they furnish critical tests for such a theory. Explanatory models in linguistics, their implications concerning the nature of cognitive processes. Chomsky

*23.756. INTRODUCTION TO LINGUISTICS IV: UNIVERSAL GRAMMAR. The nature of linguistic universals, which make it possible for languages to differ and place limits on these differences. Study of selected problem areas which show how data from particular languages contribute to the development of a strong theory of universal grammar and how such a theory dictates solutions to traditional problems in the syntax of particular languages. Perlmutter

*23.759. WORKSHOP IN SYNTAX. An intensive group tutorial seminar for discussion of research being conducted by participants. Ross

23.761. INTRODUCTION TO LINGUISTICS II: PHONOLOGY. Aims and principles of a scientific description of the phonic aspect of language. The mechanism and acoustics of speech. Distinctive features as the descriptive framework. The phoneme. The phonemic system of a language. The interrelations between phonology and other linguistic levels. Some problems of historical phonology. Practical exercises. Halle

*23.762. ADVANCED PHONOLOGY. The interrelation of syntactic, morphological, and phonological phenomena in the English language. The phonological system and the structure of phonemes. Halle

*23.764. THEORY OF PHONOLOGY. A survey of current issues in phonology. Major topics covered: ordering (disjunctivity, the cycle, local ordering, iterative vs. simultaneous rule application); rule/output relations (opacity, the abstractness issue, paradigm effects, conspiracies, the treatment of exceptions, variable rules); natural phonology (markedness, natural processes and hierarchies, the syllable, fast speech phenomena). Special attention given to the utilization of external evidence: sound change, loanwords, poetics, orthographic practice, language acquisition, psycholinguistic experiments. Kiparsky

23.766. LABORATORY ON THE PHYSIOLOGY, ACOUSTICS AND PERCEPTION OF SPEECH. Experimental investigations of speech processes. Topics include (a) interpretation of x-ray motion pictures, (b) measurement of pressure and volume velocity, (c) computer-aided waveform analysis and spectral analysis of speech, (d) synthesis of speech with an articulatory model and with a terminal analog model, (e) identification and discrimination of speechlike sounds, (f) the perceptual reality of phonetic features, and other topics. Klatt

23.768. SPEECH COMMUNICATION. Survey of structural properties of natural languages with special emphasis on the sound pattern. Survey of physiology of speech production, articulatory phonetics. Acoustical theory of speech production: the vocal tract as a generator of acoustical signals; acoustical correlates of vocal-tract configurations, both stationary and dynamic; acoustical and articulatory descriptions of phonetic features. Perception of speech: the auditory capabilities of man; evidence for perceptual correlates of phonetic categories. Mechanical recognition and generation of speech. Halle, Stevens

*23.769. WORKSHOP IN PHONOLOGY. An intensive group tutorial/seminar for discussion of research being conducted by participants. Kiparsky

23.771. MATHEMATICAL BACKGROUNDS FOR COMMUNICATION SCIENCES. Fundamentals of discrete mathematics for non-mathematicians. Introduction to various topics in set theory and foundations, logic and formal systems, modern algebra, theory of automata and computability. Staff

23.772. MATHEMATICAL MODELS IN LINGUISTICS. Study of mathematical models that relate to linguistic competence and performance. Formal properties of grammatical systems. Grammars of natural and artificial languages. Models for the speaker and hearer that incorporate representations of his knowledge of the language. Probabilistic models for the user. Stress on interconnections of theory of grammar and theory of automata, and implications for the study of human cognitive processes wherever possible. Staff

23.782. LINGUISTIC CHANGE. Review of instances of change in phonology, morphology, and syntax. Examples to be drawn from various language families, Indo-European as well as others. An attempt to develop a general theory of linguistic change. Kiparsky, Hale, Harris

23.792. SURVEY OF GENERAL LINGUISTICS. An expository and critical survey of modern structural linguistics. The work of Boas, Sapir, Bloomfield, the Prague Circle, and other developments in general linguistics during the past half-century. Contemporary trends and their immediate backgrounds. Kiparsky

Note that LINGUISTICS AND LITERARY STUDY and HISTORY OF THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE are offered jointly with the Humanities Department; SEMANTICS and ADVANCED SEMANTICS are offered jointly with the Philosophy Department; and LABORATORY ON THE PHYSIOLOGY, ACOUSTICS AND PERCEPTION OF SPEECH and SPEECH COMMUNICATION are offered jointly with the Department of Electrical Engineering.

Other courses directly relevant to linguistics include:

9.59. PSYCHOLOGY OF LANGUAGE AND COMMUNICATION. (Undergraduate) Study of psychology of language processes in light of recent advances in structural linguistics; extensive critique of learning-theoretic accounts of verbal behavior; examination of problems of language acquisition and speech perception. Methodological implications of psycholinguistic experiments and theory will be stressed. Garrett

9.591. SEMINAR IN PSYCHOLOGY OF LANGUAGE AND COMMUNICATION I. Reading, discussion, and criticism of selected topics. Emphasis on one or more of the following themes in a particular term: theories of language, speech perception, communication in subhuman species, aphasia, language acquisition, language and thought, interaction between grammatical structure and verbal performance. Garrett

9.592. SEMINAR IN PSYCHOLOGY OF LANGUAGE AND COMMUNICATION II. Advanced topics in psycholinguistics.

24.727. LOGIC AND LANGUAGE. An investigation of the relation between the syntactic and semantic representations of natural language sentences provided by generative grammars, and the formal representations provided by standard logical theories. Some, at least, of the following topics: quantifiers, opaque contexts, specificity, tense, connectives, modal operators, the notion of “logical form,” “generative” and “interpretive” accounts of sentences, definitions. Acquaintance with the standard works in generative syntax and analytic philosophy of language will be presupposed. Bromberger

To describe the research objectives at MIT in 1973, we shall quote from a mimeographed report prepared by the staff in that year entitled “The M.I.T. Program in Linguistics”:

Research Objectives and Program

The central aim of the activities of our group is to develop a general theory that reveals the law-governed relationships existing within the structure of a given language and the principles that constrain the form of any given natural languages. … It is…toward an understanding of the nature and capacities of man’s intellect that our investigations are ultimately directed. What makes language a particularly favorable starting point for investigations into man’s mental capabilities is that no other manifestation of these capacities is understood in as great detail and to such a degree of precision …

The specificity and detail of linguistic description is related to a theoretical apparatus of considerable complexity and power. … [V]ery severe limitations [are imposed] on how particular facts are to be characterized in a linguistic description. They allow us to deduce consequences beyond the facts that led to formulation of the rules in the first place, and to confirm or disconfirm the principles by testing these empirical consequences. Existence of such a theoretical apparatus is one of the distinctive features of a developed science, and linguistics shows signs of progress in this direction.

Another characteristic feature of developed science is that answers to the most central and profound questions need not be sought directly, but can instead be approached indirectly. …

Much of the energy of researchers is directed toward solving all sorts of questions of detail, because the point has been reached where the answers to questions of detail provide insight into deep questions of the nature of language. As in the natural sciences, we find in linguistics that not all questions of detail are equally rewarding. Thus, for instance, the study of palindromes…is hardly likely to be as rewarding as the study of phrases differentiated by stress contours… The palindromes…have no known bearing on any theoretical question of importance. …

It is almost immediately obvious that rules encountered in different languages exhibit striking resemblances, and these resemblances must be made explicit. To the extent that this enterprise is successful, we have at our disposal a set of general principles which tell us what sorts of properties the rules of any human language may have. It is reasonable to postulate that these general principles reflect intrinsic properties of the human “language faculty” – that they constitute an element of the innate capacity to learn and use language. …

These principles that limit the possible hypothetical rules and systems of rules cannot be learned; rather, they serve as the preconditions for learning. It is reasonable to suppose that they form part of the innate capacity of every normal child. It is these principles that constitute the theoretical apparatus of linguistics mentioned above. And since these principles are part of the natural endowment of all men, an understanding of the principles must be part of our understanding of man’s mental makeup.

(1) Theory of Grammar

…The theory of grammar is concerned with the overall structure of grammars, the question of evaluating grammars and determining their empirical adequacy. It attempts thus to delineate the innate knowledge that allows children to master languages.

(2) Formal Languages

…In theoretical syntax we are concerned with formulating the general properties of the recursive rules that relate sound to meaning. It is here that we consider such questions as whether a sharp dividing line can be drawn between semantics and syntax, whether there is justification for postulating a separate level of deep structure, whether devices other than the familiar transformation should be appealed to in accounting for specific linguistic facts, etc. The supporting evidence for these theoretical speculations comes from the study of the syntax of particular languages. The syntax of a fair number of languages has been investigated …

(4) Semantics

Semantic theory is concerned with the linguistic universals that determine semantic interpretations. It seeks to discover an abstract system of concepts that represent the “building blocks” out of which meanings are constructed, the formal relations into which these concepts can enter in semantic representations, and the character of the rules that relate semantic representations to syntactic structures. It is also concerned with the definition of general semantic properties and relations… Another line of inquiry is directed to developing a “rationalist” response to empiricist skepticism with regard to a priori knowledge. …

An early hypothesis…held that the “semantic component” of the grammar assigns to each deep structure a semantic interpretation that represents the logical form of sentences… More recent work has explored the possible contribution of other aspects of syntactic structure to semantic interpretation, in particular, in connection with anaphora, scope of logical elements, focus and presupposition, and related matters. Some linguists have proposed that there is no separate semantic component in language, but that the transformational rules themselves directly relate semantic interpretations to surface structure, with no intermediate level of deep structure. A crucial issue is the structure of the lexicon and the rules that insert lexical items into derivations. Topics in this general area are now the subject of lively discussion and intensive research. Within our program, many points of view are represented.

…The central problems of phonology are to characterize the rules that constitute this [input-output] system and to define the manner in which the rules may function. …The study of the primitives of phonology directly involves questions that are on the borderline between linguistics and acoustics, motor physiology, psychology of perception, and neuroanatomy. Work here is conducted in active collaboration with Professor K. N. Stevens of the Electrical Engineering faculty and other members of the research group.

The changes that grammars of language undergo provide important insights into the nature of the grammars themselves. … In addition, the empirical facts discovered…have a direct interest of their own, for they allow us to relate to one another languages that at present are spoken in widely separate corners of the earth and to reconstruct previous stages of the language …

… A number of studies have been undertaken during the past decade to improve our understanding of the aims, methods, and achievements of linguists of earlier generations. …

… In the past, studies in this area by people at MIT have been limited largely to metrics. Significant advances in our understanding of the nature of meter have already been made, and further work in this area is continuing. The crucial insight here was the realization that the relation between phoneme and sound is entirely parallel to that between metrical constituent and sound, and that as the former relation is much more abstract and less direct that had been generally believed possible, so is the latter. …

Work in psycholinguistics proceeds within the Department of Psychology, under joint auspices of the Department of Psychology and the Research Laboratory of Electronics. This work has been primarily directed toward the construction and evaluation of psychological models of syntax recognition and of speech development. Work in the first area has included a variety of studies of the interaction between syntactic formalisms and the sentence recognition heuristics employed by speaker/hearers in recovering the structure of messages in their language. Work in the second area has been concerned with establishing the ontogenetic sequence of syntactic development and, recently, with the development of experimental techniques for assessing the operation of constancies in phonetic perception by 3- to 5-month-old infants.

American Indian languages provide one of the richest sources of data for the linguist. It has been a matter of considerable concern to many of us that the study of these languages has been largely in the hands of outsiders, that there are practically no scholars who are native speakers of these languages. …

…[W]e are in the process of developing a new program for the study of American Indian languages. The central aim of this program is to train a number of speakers of American Indian languages in the techniques of linguistics. The most immediate result of this training is a tremendous increase in the value of the observations that one obtains from the informant… Moreover, the training which he obtains here is such as to be of value to him when he returns to his people… Only those indigenous nations or tribes that manage to hold on to their culture though the medium of language will survive. And it is here that a person with training in linguistics has an obvious and most positive role to play. …

(11) Studies in Philosophy

Work in the area of the philosophy of language is being carried out under the guidance of faculty members of the MIT Department of Philosophy. …

Doctoral Dissertations Accepted by the MIT Department to January 1974

Leave a reply cancel reply.

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.

Suggestions or feedback?

MIT News | Massachusetts Institute of Technology

  • Machine learning
  • Social justice
  • Black holes
  • Classes and programs

Departments

  • Aeronautics and Astronautics
  • Brain and Cognitive Sciences
  • Architecture
  • Political Science
  • Mechanical Engineering

Centers, Labs, & Programs

  • Abdul Latif Jameel Poverty Action Lab (J-PAL)
  • Picower Institute for Learning and Memory
  • Lincoln Laboratory
  • School of Architecture + Planning
  • School of Engineering
  • School of Humanities, Arts, and Social Sciences
  • Sloan School of Management
  • School of Science
  • MIT Schwarzman College of Computing

MIT senior Christine Soh integrates computer science and linguistics

Press contact :.

Potential applications of Soh's work in computational linguistics include improving speech recognition software and making machine-produced speech sound more natural.

Previous image Next image

Christine Soh fell in love with MIT the summer before her senior year of high school while attending the Women’s Technology Program run by MIT’s Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science. That’s when she discovered that learning to program in Python is just like learning a new language — and Soh loves languages. Growing up in Colorado, Soh spoke both English and Korean; she learned French and Latin in school. This June, Soh will graduate from MIT, where she has happily combined her passions by majoring in computer science and engineering (Course 6-3) and linguistics (Course 24). She plans to begin working toward a PhD in linguistics next year. With fluency in both technical and humanistic modes of thinking, Soh exemplifies a "bilingual" perspective. "Dual competence is a good model for undergraduates at MIT," says engineer/historian David Mindell, who encourages MIT students to "master two fundamental ways of thinking about the world, one technical and one humanistic or social. Sometimes these two modes will be at odds with each other, which raises critical questions. Other times they will be synergistic and energizing."   The challenge of natural language and computation “The really cool thing about language is that it’s universal,” says Soh, who has added ancient Greek, Chinese, and the programming language Java to her credits since that summer. “I can have a really interesting conversation with anybody, even if they don’t have a linguistics background, because everyone has experience with language.” That said, natural language is difficult for computers to comprehend — something Soh finds fascinating. “It’s really interesting to think about how we understand language,” she says. “How is it that computers have such a hard time understanding what we find so easy?” Tools from computational linguistics to improve speech Pairing linguistics with computer science has allowed Soh to explore cutting-edge research combining the two disciplines. Thanks to MIT’s Advanced Undergraduate Research Opportunities Program, Soh got the chance to explore whether speech analysis software can be used as a tool for the clinical diagnosis of speech impairments.

“It’s very difficult to correctly diagnose a child because a speech impairment can be caused by a ton of different things,” says Soh. Working with the Speech Communication Group in MIT’s Research Laboratory of Electronics, Soh has been developing a tool that can listen to a child’s speech and extract linguistic information, such where in the mouth the sound was produced, thus identifying modifications from the proper formation of the word. “We can then use computational techniques to see if there are patterns to the modifications that have been made and see if these patterns can distinguish one underlying condition from another.” A natural leader

Even if the team isn’t able to find such patterns, Soh says the tool could be used by speech pathologists to learn more about what linguistic modifications a child might need to make to improve speech. In December, Soh presented a poster on this work at the annual meeting of the Acoustical Society of America and was honored with a first-place prize in her category (signal processing in acoustics). Exploring such real-world applications for computational linguistics helped inspire Soh to apply to doctoral programs in linguistics for next year. “I’ll be doing research that will be integrating computer science and linguistics,” she says, noting that possible applications of computational linguistics include working to improve speech-recognition software or to make machine-produced speech sound more natural. “I look forward to using the knowledge and skills I’ve learned at MIT in doing that research.” “Christine’s unique interests, energy, and deep interests in both linguistics and computer science should enable her to accomplish great things,” says Suzanne Flynn, a professor of linguistics who has had Soh as a student. “She is a natural leader.”   From field methods to neurolinguistics Looking back at her time at MIT, Soh recalls particularly enjoying two linguistics classes: 24.909 (Field Methods in Linguistics) which explores the structure of an unfamiliar language through direct work with a native speaker (in Soh’s year, the class centered on Wolof, which is spoken in Senegal, the Gambia, and Mauritania), and 24.906 (The Linguistic Study of Bilingualism). In the latter class, Soh says, “We looked at neurolinguistics, what’s happening in the brain as the bilingual brain developed. We looked at topics in sociolinguistics: In communities that are bilingual, like Quebec, what kind of impact does it have on society, such as how schools are run? … We got to see a spectrum of linguistics. It was really cool.” Building community at MIT Outside class, Soh says she found community at MIT through the Asian Christian Fellowship and the Society of Women Engineers (SWE), which she served last year as vice president of membership. “SWE has also been a really awesome community and has opened up opportunities for conversation about what it means to be a woman engineer,” she says. Interestingly, Soh almost didn’t apply to MIT at all, simply because her brother was already at the Institute. (Albert Soh ’18 is now a high school teacher of math and physics.) Fortunately, the Women’s Technology Program changed her mind, and as she nears graduation, Soh says, "MIT has been absolutely fantastic.”  

Story prepared by MIT SHASS Communications Editorial and Design Director: Emily Hiestand Senior Writer: Kathryn O'Neill  

Share this news article on:, related links.

  • Christine Soh - SuperUROP project
  • MIT Linguistics
  • Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science
  • Women's Technology Program
  • MIT Asian Christian Fellowship

Related Topics

  • School of Humanities Arts and Social Sciences
  • Electrical engineering and computer science (EECS)
  • Research Laboratory of Electronics
  • computer science
  • Linguistics
  • Women in STEM

Related Articles

Junior Alex Cuellar with his constructed language. The chalkboard reads: "I can speak Oafal."

How to build a language

In a pair of studies, researchers show that grammar-enriched deep learning models understand some key rules about language use. Peng Qian (left) and Ethan Wilcox, graduate students at MIT and Harvard University respectively, presented the work at a recent MIT-IBM Watson AI Lab poster session.

Teaching language models grammar really does make them smarter

mit phd in linguistics

3 Questions: What is linguistics?

Previous item Next item

More MIT News

6x6 grid of purple squares containing yellow shapes representing phonon stability boundaries. A diagonal row of squares from top left to bottom right shows graphical maps of the boundaries.

A first-ever complete map for elastic strain engineering

Read full story →

Oil field rigs overlayed with analytics data

Shining a light on oil fields to make them more sustainable

Rafael Jaramillo sits in his office and looks to the side. A large wrench sits on the window sill. The desk is covered in white paper with many drawings and notes on it.

“Life is short, so aim high”

Three close up photos of speakers at a conference: Julie Shah, Ben Armstrong, and Kate Kellogg

MIT launches Working Group on Generative AI and the Work of the Future

Two men in hardhats and safety vests, seen from behind, inspect a forest of electrical pylons and wires on a cloudless day

Atmospheric observations in China show rise in emissions of a potent greenhouse gas

A view of the steps and columns of 77 Mass Ave, as seen through The Alchemist Sculpture. Glimpses of the numbers and mathematical symbols are seen around the image.

Second round of seed grants awarded to MIT scholars studying the impact and applications of generative AI

  • More news on MIT News homepage →

Massachusetts Institute of Technology 77 Massachusetts Avenue, Cambridge, MA, USA

  • Map (opens in new window)
  • Events (opens in new window)
  • People (opens in new window)
  • Careers (opens in new window)
  • Accessibility
  • Social Media Hub
  • MIT on Facebook
  • MIT on YouTube
  • MIT on Instagram

Yadav Gowda

I’m a fifth-year PhD student in the Department of Linguistics and Philosophy at MIT .

I’m interested in syntax and the syntax-prosody interface, especially how prosody and word order interact. Right now I’m interested in linear word order in so-called “mixed headedness languages”.

I’ve recently become interested in matters of tense and aspect, and I’ve been thinking about the role of scalar implicatures in the interpretation of tense, aspect, and inner aspect, especially in the context of my heritage language, Kannada.

I’m a member of the MIT Language Acquisition Lab , where my most recent project was with Elise Newman and Martin Hackl on the acquisition of the English scalar particle even .

Here’s some of my previous research .

I have a blog that I sometimes write things on.

In General American English, my name is pronounced /ˈjɑːdʌv ˈɡaʊ̯dʌ/ .

My CV is available here .

You can contact me at [email protected] .

  • Elise Newman , Martin Hackl , Leo Rosenstein and I published Scalar inferences in the acquisition of even .
  • Danfeng Wu and I gave a talk entitled Clitic climbing and linear adjacency in Wolof at the LSA 2020 Annual Meeting in New Orleans. [ handout ]
  • I gave a talk entitled Movement within and without a clause at FASAL 9 in Portland, on the interpretational effects of clausal complement movement in Bengali. [ handout ]

Ph.D. Programs

The Department of Linguistics offers four concentrations leading to the Doctor of Philosophy (Ph.D.) degree in Linguistics (see list below). No matter the concentration, our faculty work closely with students, guiding their research and supporting their passions.

  • Applied Linguistics
  • Computational Linguistics
  • Sociolinguistics
  • Theoretical Linguistics

Applicants to the Ph.D. program are encouraged to identify prospective research advisors, at least one of whom should be in the concentration to which they apply.

After entering the program, Ph.D. students may elect to add a minor in a second one of these concentrations [new policy effective Spring 2023].

An interdisciplinary (second) concentration in Cognitive Science is also available to Ph.D. students.

Master’s in Passing

If, in their course of the Ph.D. program, a doctoral student meets all of the requirements of a M.S. degree in Linguistics, he or she may apply to receive a “Master’s in Passing.” Please consult section IV.D.3 of the Graduate School Bulletin for full details about the “in passing” or “terminal” Master’s degree.

  • Skip to Content
  • Bulletin Home

MIT Bulletin

  • Degree Charts >
  • Linguistics (SM)
  • Around Campus
  • Academic Program
  • Administration
  • Arts at MIT
  • Campus Media
  • Fraternities, Sororities, and Independent Living Groups
  • Medical Services
  • Priscilla King Gray Public Service Center
  • Religious Organizations
  • Student Government
  • Work/​Life and Family Resources
  • Advising and Support
  • Digital Learning
  • Disability and Access Services
  • Information Systems and Technology
  • Student Financial Services
  • Writing and Communication Center
  • Major Course of Study
  • General Institute Requirements
  • Independent Activites Period
  • Undergraduate Research Opportunities Program
  • First-​Year Advising Seminars
  • Interphase EDGE/​x
  • Edgerton Center
  • Grading Options
  • Study at Other Universities
  • Internships Abroad
  • Career Advising and Professional Development
  • Teacher Licensure and Education
  • ROTC Programs
  • Financial Aid
  • Medical Requirements
  • Graduate Study at MIT
  • General Degree Requirements
  • Other Institutions
  • Registration
  • Term Regulations and Examination Policies
  • Academic Performance and Grades
  • Policies and Procedures
  • Privacy of Student Records
  • Abdul Latif Jameel Poverty Action Lab
  • Art, Culture, and Technology Program
  • Broad Institute of MIT and Harvard
  • Center for Archaeological Materials
  • Center for Bits and Atoms
  • Center for Clinical and Translational Research
  • Center for Collective Intelligence
  • Center for Computational Science and Engineering
  • Center for Constructive Communication
  • Center for Energy and Environmental Policy Research
  • Center for Environmental Health Sciences
  • Center for Global Change Science
  • Center for International Studies
  • Center for Real Estate
  • Center for Transportation &​ Logistics
  • Computer Science and Artificial Intelligence Laboratory
  • Concrete Sustainability Hub
  • D-​Lab
  • Deshpande Center for Technological Innovation
  • Division of Comparative Medicine
  • Haystack Observatory
  • Initiative on the Digital Economy
  • Institute for Medical Engineering and Science
  • Institute for Soldier Nanotechnologies
  • Institute for Work and Employment Research
  • Internet Policy Research Initiative
  • Joint Program on the Science and Policy of Global Change
  • Knight Science Journalism Program
  • Koch Institute for Integrative Cancer Research
  • Laboratory for Financial Engineering
  • Laboratory for Information and Decision Systems
  • Laboratory for Manufacturing and Productivity
  • Laboratory for Nuclear Science
  • Legatum Center for Development and Entrepreneurship
  • Lincoln Laboratory
  • Martin Trust Center for MIT Entrepreneurship
  • Materials Research Laboratory
  • McGovern Institute for Brain Research
  • Microsystems Technology Laboratories
  • MIT Center for Art, Science &​ Technology
  • MIT Energy Initiative
  • MIT Environmental Solutions Initiative
  • MIT Kavli Institute for Astrophysics and Space Research
  • MIT Media Lab
  • MIT Office of Innovation
  • MIT Open Learning
  • MIT Portugal Program
  • MIT Professional Education
  • MIT Sea Grant College Program
  • Nuclear Reactor Laboratory
  • Operations Research Center
  • Picower Institute for Learning and Memory
  • Plasma Science and Fusion Center
  • Research Laboratory of Electronics
  • Simons Center for the Social Brain
  • Singapore-​MIT Alliance for Research and Technology Centre
  • Sociotechnical Systems Research Center
  • Whitehead Institute for Biomedical Research
  • Women's and Gender Studies Program
  • Architecture (Course 4)
  • Art and Design (Course 4-​B)
  • Art, Culture, and Technology (SM)
  • Media Arts and Sciences
  • Planning (Course 11)
  • Urban Science and Planning with Computer Science (Course 11-​6)
  • Aerospace Engineering (Course 16)
  • Engineering (Course 16-​ENG)
  • Biological Engineering (Course 20)
  • Chemical Engineering (Course 10)
  • Chemical-​Biological Engineering (Course 10-​B)
  • Chemical Engineering (Course 10-​C)
  • Engineering (Course 10-​ENG)
  • Engineering (Course 1-​ENG)
  • Electrical Engineering and Computer Science (Course 6-​2)
  • Electrical Science and Engineering (Course 6-​1)
  • Computation and Cognition (Course 6-​9)
  • Computer Science and Engineering (Course 6-​3)
  • Computer Science and Molecular Biology (Course 6-​7)
  • Electrical Engineering and Computer Science (MEng)
  • Computer Science and Molecular Biology (MEng)
  • Health Sciences and Technology
  • Archaeology and Materials (Course 3-​C)
  • Materials Science and Engineering (Course 3)
  • Materials Science and Engineering (Course 3-​A)
  • Materials Science and Engineering (PhD)
  • Mechanical Engineering (Course 2)
  • Mechanical and Ocean Engineering (Course 2-​OE)
  • Engineering (Course 2-​A)
  • Nuclear Science and Engineering (Course 22)
  • Engineering (Course 22-​ENG)
  • Anthropology (Course 21A)
  • Comparative Media Studies (CMS)
  • Writing (Course 21W)
  • Economics (Course 14-​1)
  • Mathematical Economics (Course 14-​2)
  • Data, Economics, and Design of Policy (MASc)
  • Economics (PhD)
  • Global Studies and Languages (Course 21G)
  • History (Course 21H)
  • Linguistics and Philosophy (Course 24-​2)
  • Philosophy (Course 24-​1)
  • Literature (Course 21L)
  • Music (Course 21M-​1)
  • Theater Arts (Course 21M-​2)
  • Political Science (Course 17)
  • Science, Technology, and Society/​Second Major (STS)
  • Business Analytics (Course 15-​2)
  • Finance (Course 15-​3)
  • Management (Course 15-​1)
  • Biology (Course 7)
  • Chemistry and Biology (Course 5-​7)
  • Brain and Cognitive Sciences (Course 9)
  • Chemistry (Course 5)
  • Earth, Atmospheric and Planetary Sciences (Course 12)
  • Mathematics (Course 18)
  • Mathematics with Computer Science (Course 18-​C)
  • Physics (Course 8)
  • Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science
  • Institute for Data, Systems, and Society
  • Chemistry and Biology
  • Climate System Science and Engineering
  • Computation and Cognition
  • Computer Science and Molecular Biology
  • Computer Science, Economics, and Data Science
  • Humanities and Engineering
  • Humanities and Science
  • Urban Science and Planning with Computer Science
  • African and African Diaspora Studies
  • American Studies
  • Ancient and Medieval Studies
  • Applied International Studies
  • Asian and Asian Diaspora Studies
  • Biomedical Engineering
  • Energy Studies
  • Entrepreneurship and Innovation
  • Environment and Sustainability
  • Latin American and Latino/​a Studies
  • Middle Eastern Studies
  • Polymers and Soft Matter
  • Public Policy
  • Russian and Eurasian Studies
  • Statistics and Data Science
  • Women's and Gender Studies
  • Advanced Urbanism
  • Computational and Systems Biology
  • Computational Science and Engineering
  • Design and Management (IDM &​ SDM)
  • Joint Program with Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution
  • Leaders for Global Operations
  • Microbiology
  • Music Technology and Computation
  • Operations Research
  • Real Estate Development
  • Social and Engineering Systems
  • Supply Chain Management
  • Technology and Policy
  • Transportation
  • School of Architecture and Planning
  • School of Engineering
  • Aeronautics and Astronautics Fields (PhD)
  • Artificial Intelligence and Decision Making (Course 6-​4)
  • Biological Engineering (PhD)
  • Nuclear Science and Engineering (PhD)
  • School of Humanities, Arts, and Social Sciences
  • Humanities (Course 21)
  • Humanities and Engineering (Course 21E)
  • Humanities and Science (Course 21S)
  • Sloan School of Management
  • School of Science
  • Brain and Cognitive Sciences (PhD)
  • Earth, Atmospheric and Planetary Sciences Fields (PhD)
  • Interdisciplinary Programs (SB)
  • Climate System Science and Engineering (Course 1-​12)
  • Computer Science, Economics, and Data Science (Course 6-​14)
  • Interdisciplinary Programs (Graduate)
  • Computation and Cognition (MEng)
  • Computational Science and Engineering (SM)
  • Computational Science and Engineering (PhD)
  • Computer Science, Economics, and Data Science (MEng)
  • Leaders for Global Operations (MBA/​SM and SM)
  • Music Technology and Computation (SM and MASc)
  • Real Estate Development (SM)
  • Statistics (PhD)
  • Supply Chain Management (MEng and MASc)
  • Technology and Policy (SM)
  • Transportation (SM)
  • Aeronautics and Astronautics (Course 16)
  • Aerospace Studies (AS)
  • Civil and Environmental Engineering (Course 1)
  • Comparative Media Studies /​ Writing (CMS)
  • Comparative Media Studies /​ Writing (Course 21W)
  • Computational and Systems Biology (CSB)
  • Computational Science and Engineering (CSE)
  • Concourse (CC)
  • Data, Systems, and Society (IDS)
  • Earth, Atmospheric, and Planetary Sciences (Course 12)
  • Economics (Course 14)
  • Edgerton Center (EC)
  • Electrical Engineering and Computer Science (Course 6)
  • Engineering Management (EM)
  • Experimental Study Group (ES)
  • Global Languages (Course 21G)
  • Health Sciences and Technology (HST)
  • Linguistics and Philosophy (Course 24)
  • Management (Course 15)
  • Media Arts and Sciences (MAS)
  • Military Science (MS)
  • Music and Theater Arts (Course 21M)
  • Naval Science (NS)
  • Science, Technology, and Society (STS)
  • Special Programs
  • Supply Chain Management (SCM)
  • Urban Studies and Planning (Course 11)
  • Women's and Gender Studies (WGS)

Master of Science in Linguistics

Department of Linguistics and Philosophy

Program Requirements

MIT Academic Bulletin

Print this page.

The PDF includes all information on this page and its related tabs. Subject (course) information includes any changes approved for the current academic year.

ISO Home

Degree programs

Mit offers a wide range of degrees and programs..

All graduate students, whether or not they are participating in an interdepartmental program, must have a primary affiliation with and be registered in a single department. Every applicant accepted by MIT is admitted through one of the graduate departments. MIT has a number of established interdepartmental programs, and there are many more opportunities for students to arrange interdepartmental programs with interested faculty members.

All MIT graduate degree programs have residency requirements, which reflect academic terms (excluding summer). Some degrees also require completion of an acceptable thesis prepared in residence at MIT, unless special permission is granted for part of the thesis work to be accomplished elsewhere. Other degrees require a pro-seminar or capstone experience.

Applicants interested in graduate education should apply to the department or graduate program conducting research in the area of interest. Below is an alphabetical list of all the available departments and programs that offer a graduate-level degree.

Interested in reading first-hand accounts of MIT graduate students from a variety of programs? Visit the Grad Blog . Prospective students who want to talk with a current student can reach out to their department(s) of interest for connections or, if they are interested in the MIT experience for diverse communities, can reach out to a GradDiversity Ambassador .

Search Programs

This site uses cookies to give you the best possible experience. By browsing our website, you agree to our use of cookies.

If you require further information, please visit the Privacy Policy page.

  • DACA/Undocumented
  • First Generation, Low Income
  • International Students
  • Students of Color
  • Students with disabilities
  • Undergraduate Students
  • Master’s Students
  • PhD Students
  • Faculty/Staff
  • Family/Supporters
  • Career Fairs
  • Post Jobs, Internships, Fellowships
  • Build your Brand at MIT
  • Recruiting Guidelines and Resources
  • Connect with Us
  • Career Advising
  • Distinguished Fellowships
  • Employer Relations
  • Graduate Student Professional Development
  • Prehealth Advising
  • Student Leadership Opportunities
  • Academia & Education
  • Architecture, Planning, & Design
  • Arts, Communications, & Media
  • Business, Finance, & Fintech
  • Computing & Computer Technology
  • Data Science
  • Energy, Environment, & Sustainability
  • Life Sciences, Biotech, & Pharma
  • Manufacturing & Transportation
  • Health & Medical Professions
  • Social Impact, Policy, & Law
  • Getting Started & Handshake 101
  • Exploring careers
  • Networking & Informational Interviews
  • Connecting with employers
  • Resumes, cover letters, portfolios, & CVs
  • Finding a Job or Internship
  • Post-Graduate and Summer Outcomes
  • Professional Development Competencies
  • Preparing for Graduate & Professional Schools
  • Preparing for Medical / Health Profession Schools
  • Interviewing
  • New jobs & career transitions
  • Career Prep and Development Programs
  • Employer Events
  • Outside Events for Career and Professional Development
  • Events Calendar
  • Career Services Workshop Requests
  • Early Career Advisory Board
  • Peer Career Advisors
  • Student Staff
  • Mission, Vision, Values and Diversity Commitments
  • News and Reports

Johns Hopkins University Applied Physics Laboratory

2024 phd graduate – ontologist.

  • Share This: Share 2024 PhD Graduate – Ontologist on Facebook Share 2024 PhD Graduate – Ontologist on LinkedIn Share 2024 PhD Graduate – Ontologist on X

Do you love to build ontologies?

Are you passionate about semantic information systems?

If so, we’re looking for someone like you to join our team at APL.

We are seeking an ontologist. As a member of our team, you’ll contribute to ontologies in a variety of domains. You will be joining a hardworking team of engineers and scientists who craft and implement novel methods for solving meaningful national security challenges. We are passionate about applying knowledge representation techniques to draw insights from and enable computational analysis of large sets of structured and unstructured data.

As an Ontologist

  • Your primary responsibility will be to build ontologies.
  • You will engage stakeholders to determine ontology requirements.
  • You will work with subject matter experts to craft ontology content.
  • You will contribute to quality assurance and validation practices.

You meet our minimum qualifications for the job if you…

  • Possess a PhD degree in a field related to Applied Ontology (e.g. Philosophy, Computer Science, Cognitive Science, Mathematics, Logic, or Linguistics)
  • Are proficient with Web Ontology Language (OWL2), Resource Description Framework Schema (RDFs), and Resource Description Framework (RDF)
  • Are proficient with SPARQL
  • Have a proven track record of conducting independent research within the field of ontology and knowledge representation
  • Have excellent written and oral communication skills, and the ability to work with a wide range of collaborators
  • Have experience building domain ontologies
  • Are proficient with ontology editors (e.g., TopBraid Composer, Protégé)
  • Have experience with formal representations of logical reasoning and inference
  • Have experience eliciting, defining, and communicating modeling requirements for domain before crafting ontologies that meet those requirements
  • Have experience with multi- or intra-disciplinary collaboration.
  • Are able to obtain a Secret security clearance. If selected, you will be subject to a government security clearance investigation and must meet the requirements for access to classified information. Eligibility requirements include U.S. citizenship.

You’ll exceed our minimum requirements if you have…

  • A Doctorate degree in a field related to Applied Ontology (e.g. Philosophy, Computer Science, Cognitive Science, Mathematics, Logic, or Linguistics)
  • Experience working with a software engineering team to deliver ontology-driven software features
  • Experience building domain ontologies based on a top-level ontology
  • Comfort with weighing tradeoffs between semantic expressivity and project-specific implementation strategies
  • Experience with any of the following: Basic Formal Ontology (BFO); The Common Core Ontologies (CCO); W3C standards such as SOSA/SSN and the Time Ontology; ontologies in the Open Biological and Biomedical (OBO) Foundry; as well as other semantic web standards, such as Dublin Core, SKOS, QUDT, and GeoSPARQL
  • Experience with triple stores or graph databases (e.g., GraphDB, Stardog, Jena Fuseki)
  • Experience with semantic software packages and frameworks (e.g., Jena, Sesame, RDF4J, RDFLib)
  • Experience with semantic validation languages (e.g. SHACL, SPIN)
  • Experience with software version management using GIT
  • Experience with Python
  • Experience applying ontologies in natural language processing, image processing, or in the normalization/integration of heterogeneous data sources.
  • Coursework in data structures, databases, metaphysics, knowledge representation, algorithms, mathematical logic, mereotopology, or software programming

Why work at APL?

The Johns Hopkins University Applied Physics Laboratory (APL) brings world-class expertise to our nation’s most critical defense, security, space and science challenges. While we are dedicated to solving complex challenges and pioneering new technologies, what makes us truly outstanding is our culture. We offer a vibrant, welcoming atmosphere where you can bring your authentic self to work, continue to grow, and build strong connections with inspiring teammates.

At APL, we celebrate our differences and encourage creativity and bold, new ideas. Our employees enjoy generous benefits, including a robust education assistance program, unparalleled retirement contributions, and a healthy work/life balance. APL’s campus is located in the Baltimore-Washington metro area. Learn more about our career opportunities at http://www.jhuapl.edu/careers.

APL is an Equal Opportunity/Affirmative Action employer. All qualified applicants will receive consideration for employment without regard to race, creed, color, religion, sex, gender identity or expression, sexual orientation, national origin, age, physical or mental disability, genetic information, veteran status, occupation, marital or familial status, political opinion, personal appearance, or any other characteristic protected by applicable law.

APL is committed to promoting an innovative environment that embraces diversity, encourages creativity, and supports inclusion of new ideas. In doing so, we are committed to providing reasonable accommodation to individuals of all abilities, including those with disabilities. If you require a reasonable accommodation to participate in any part of the hiring process, please contact [email protected]. Only by ensuring that everyone’s voice is heard are we empowered to be bold, do great things, and make the world a better place.

IMAGES

  1. MIT senior Christine Soh integrates computer science and linguistics

    mit phd in linguistics

  2. Mit Press: Linguistics: An Introduction to Language and Communication

    mit phd in linguistics

  3. PhD In Linguistics Vs PhD In Language: Which Is Right For You?

    mit phd in linguistics

  4. Hear the “Conlangs” Invented by MIT Linguistics Students

    mit phd in linguistics

  5. Summary of PHD in Linguistics

    mit phd in linguistics

  6. Linguistics Phd Research Proposal

    mit phd in linguistics

VIDEO

  1. PhD in Linguistics, Language Documentation and Revitalization

  2. 50 years of Linguistics at MIT, Lecture 7

  3. 50 years of Linguistics at MIT, Lecture 2

  4. 50 years of Linguistics at MIT, Lecture 4

  5. Ph.D. in Linguistics' terrible advice: "dictionaries take a back seat"

  6. Lecture 2: Mathematical Structure of Syntactic Merge

COMMENTS

  1. Graduate Program

    Graduate Program - MIT Linguistics

  2. MIT Linguistics

    MIT Linguistics - Department of Linguistics and Philosophy

  3. Linguistics

    Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics (PhD) Standardized Tests International English Language Testing System (IELTS) Minimum score required: 6.5; Electronic scores send to: MIT Graduate Admissions ... MIT Office of Graduate Education 77 Massachusetts Avenue Room 3-107 Cambridge, MA 02139-4307. Contact Us: [email protected] (617) 253-4860. MIT ...

  4. Department of Linguistics and Philosophy < MIT

    Inquiries. Information regarding undergraduate or graduate academic programs, research activities, admissions, financial aid, and assistantships may be obtained from the Department of Linguistics and Philosophy, Room 32-D808, 617-253-9372. 24.93 The Search for Meaning.

  5. Linguistics and Philosophy

    MIT Office of Graduate Education 77 Massachusetts Avenue Room 3-107 Cambridge, MA 02139-4307

  6. Graduate Admissions

    TOEFL Department codes: 20 (Philosophy) Institute code: 3514 (MIT) IELTS: Minimum score required: 6.5. Electronic scores send to: MIT Graduate Admissions. However, the Department of Linguistics and Philosophy does grant waivers. In general, we grant waivers to students who have received a degree from an American or English-speaking university ...

  7. PDF Department of Linguistics and Philosophy

    Students in the MITILI in the SM in Linguistics (htt p:// cat alog.mit .edu/degree-char t s/sm-linguistics) must fulll the Institute's General Degree Requirements (htt p://cat alog.mit .edu/ mit/graduate-educatio n/general-degree-requirement s/ #mastersdegreetext): 66 units of graduate subject credit, plus a thesis approved by the department.

  8. MIT Department of Linguistics: Research

    The MIT Linguistics Group aims to discover the rules and representations underlying the structures of languages. The program covers the subfields of linguistics (phonetics, phonology, morphology, syntax, semantics, and psycholinguistics) as well as interfaces with philosophy and logic, speech science and technology, computer science and artificial intelligence, and study of the brain and ...

  9. Doctoral Degrees

    A doctoral degree requires the satisfactory completion of an approved program of advanced study and original research of high quality. Please note that the Doctor of Philosophy (PhD) and Doctor of Science (ScD) degrees are awarded interchangeably by all departments in the School of Engineering and the School of Science, except in the fields of biology, cognitive science, neuroscience, medical ...

  10. MITWPL

    Founded in 1979, MIT Working Papers in Linguistics is an entirely student-managed organization, run by the graduate students in the MIT Doctoral Program in Linguistics. MITWPL is dedicated to promoting student linguistic research by publishing dissertations and papers and, with the proceeds, equitably funding travel, fieldwork and equipment purchases.

  11. The Development of the Linguistics Program at the Massachusetts

    The linguistics program at MIT evolved in a highly unusual manner for an academic discipline with ancient beginnings. Essentially, the linguistics section was formed not so much to teach a body of knowledge as to contribute to it. ... In September 1961 the first officially registered graduate students in linguistics entered MIT. The program ...

  12. MIT senior Christine Soh integrates computer science and linguistics

    Growing up in Colorado, Soh spoke both English and Korean; she learned French and Latin in school. This June, Soh will graduate from MIT, where she has happily combined her passions by majoring in computer science and engineering (Course 6-3) and linguistics (Course 24). She plans to begin working toward a PhD in linguistics next year.

  13. Yadav Gowda

    I'm a fifth-year PhD student in the Department of Linguistics and Philosophy at MIT. I'm interested in syntax and the syntax-prosody interface, especially how prosody and word order interact. Right now I'm interested in linear word order in so-called "mixed headedness languages". I've recently become interested in matters of tense and aspect, and I've been thinking about the role ...

  14. Linguistics, Ph.D.

    About. To be admitted to the doctoral program at Massachusetts Institute of Technology (MIT), applicants must satisfy the Institute requirements for admission to the Graduate School, and must be formally accepted by the linguistics faculty of the Department of Linguistics and Philosophy as a candidate for the Ph.D. degree.

  15. Ph.D. Programs

    The Department of Linguistics offers four concentrations leading to the Doctor of Philosophy (Ph.D.) degree in Linguistics (see list below). No matter the concentration, our faculty work closely with students, guiding their research and supporting their passions. Applicants to the Ph.D. program are encouraged to identify prospective research advisors, at least one of whom should […]

  16. Programs

    Programs - MIT Linguistics

  17. Master of Science in Linguistics < MIT

    Total Units. 120-144. 1. If the student has already completed equivalent coursework in this area, a more advanced class in the same area is taken instead. 2. Students register for a minimum of 24 units of 24.921 Independent Study: Linguistics, focused on their own language. Students can register for additional terms of 24.921.

  18. Degree programs

    MIT Sloan Master of Finance. January 4. MIT Sloan Master of Science in Management Studies. February 15. MIT Sloan MBA Program. September 29, January 18, April 11. MIT Sloan PhD Program. December 1. MIT-WHOI Joint Program in Oceanography / Applied Ocean Science and Engineering.

  19. 2024 PhD Graduate

    Possess a PhD degree in a field related to Applied Ontology (e.g. Philosophy, Computer Science, Cognitive Science, Mathematics, Logic, or Linguistics) Are proficient with Web Ontology Language (OWL2), Resource Description Framework Schema (RDFs), and Resource Description Framework (RDF) Are proficient with SPARQL