• International edition
  • Australia edition
  • Europe edition

If you’re frustrated and stressed because of your dissertation, you’re not alone.

All-nighters and self-doubt: learn from our dissertation disasters

Recent graduates recall their dissertation slip-ups and share their advice on supervisors, footnotes and steering clear of the pub

I t’s likely to be the greatest academic challenge you’ll face as a student. Speak to a finalist working in the library at the moment and you’ll see from their gaunt and despairing facial expression that writing a thesis is not a fun thing to do.

These students take us through their hair-raising experiences - and share their tips for success.

I got the flu, and had to pull three all-nighters in a row

The dissertation was “a long, arduous process” for William Lloyd, a recent journalism graduate at Kingston University. “I caught the flu for the second time in my life, a week before it was due. That wasn’t ideal because I’d not really organised my time properly.

“True to form, I had left half of it to write with a few days left. I got a small extension due to the illness but had travel back to uni from home and do three all-nighters in a row at the library in order to get it done. Bloody hell, it took its toll.

“Whatever happens, my advice is not to panic. It was quite fun, in a way.”

My supervisor told me I was ‘not a scholar’

Cat Soave, a recent English literature graduate from the University of York, says: “I immediately encountered problems with my dissertation supervisor. They decided that I couldn’t write about the topic I had spent three years of education working up to. Their rationale was that I was “not a scholar” and would be unable to do adequate research for my topic.

“I was incredibly disappointed, and had to begin my research from scratch. In later meetings, I didn’t feel confident enough to be very vocal for fear of further criticism. I ended up completing my dissertation with next to no help or direction.”

What can we draw from Cat’s experience? It’s important to build a good relationship with your supervisor or try to find a different one if it clearly isn’t going to work.

Avoid unnecessary tinkering

Alys Key, a third-year English literature and language student at the University of Oxford, says: “The biggest problem I had with my dissertation was the final stages of drafting. The more I read it, the more it seemed to have problems, even if I’d been happier at an earlier stage.

“I think the key is to set yourself a cut-off point, at least a day or two before the deadline, and just limit yourself to proofreading. Everything seems bad when you’ve read it 100 times, so you have to have a bit of faith.”

I should have looked for more interesting research material

“Looking back, I should have researched more broadly,” says Emma Guest, an English literature and film studies graduate from Worcester University.

“I wrote my dissertation on two films by Guillermo del Toro. When I was looking for secondary reading to support my essay, I mainly focused on finding books on the topic. I think some people don’t realise that there are more interesting forms of secondary reading out there – such as archived papers, documentaries, and so on.”

Different tutors wanted different things - and some didn’t care

For Rupert McCallum, 21, a third-year biological sciences student at the University of Portsmouth, formatting his essay became an obstacle. “Different tutors within the department wanted different things - and some didn’t care,” he says.

“My advice would be to read up early on how to format your essay in case it becomes a pain closer to the deadline. Then double check, especially if the department is sending mixed messages. Although some of it may seem silly, sometimes it’s best just to jump through the hoops.”

I found it was easy to get sidetracked

Jessica Shales studied Anglo Saxon, Norse, and Celtic at the University of Cambridge – a specialist subject that can be difficult to research. “I found it was quite easy to become sidetracked, and to start reading lots in detail about stuff that wasn’t directly related to my question. If I were to start again, I think I would want to keep my overall aim more clearly in mind,” she says.

“I would also start writing it later than I did. I think I panicked a bit and wanted to get something down on paper, and so my argument wasn’t properly formed when I started writing. I think I was a bit scared by the fact that the dissertation was longer than anything I’d written before.

“I suppose my advice is to do whatever you’d try to do in a shorter essay, which is to pose a question, use relevant evidence to discuss it, and arrive at a conclusion accordingly.”

There’s nothing quite so soul-destroying as losing a page reference

Kate Wallis, 21, who studies arts and siences at University College London (UCL), learned the hard way to reference as she went along. “And I mean really reference, with page numbers. I cannot emphasise this enough.

“There’s nothing quite so soul-destroying as a stack of 20 books next to you that you have to go through to work out which elusive page your trifling statistic came from,” she says. “It’s advice that probably applies to all essay writing , but the dissertation is where it really comes to the fore.”

Top tip: don’t drink and dissertate

Don’t follow the example of William Buck, 21, who studied history at Cardiff University. “A desire to be in the pub let me down a bit. I was out at a night club about five times a week,” he says.

Keep up with the latest on Guardian Students: follow us on Twitter at @GdnStudents – and become a member to receive exclusive benefits and our weekly newsletter.

  • Guardian Students
  • Higher education
  • Advice for students

Comments (…)

Most viewed.

Emotional Eating in College Students: Associations with Coping and Healthy Eating Motivators and Barriers

  • Full length manuscript
  • Published: 29 June 2023

Cite this article

i'm stress eating over my dissertation

  • Elizabeth D. Dalton   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-4013-6450 1  

1079 Accesses

Explore all metrics

Emotional eating, or eating in response to stress and other negative affective states, bears negative consequences including excessive weight gain and heightened risk of binge eating disorder. Responding to stress with emotional eating is not universal, and it is important to elucidate under what circumstances and by what mechanisms stress is associated with emotional eating. This is particularly important to understand among college students, who are at risk of experiencing heightened stress and negative changes to dietary habits.

The present study investigated the relationships among perceived stress, emotional eating, coping, and barriers to and motivators of healthy eating both concurrently and 1 year later in a sample of young adult college students ( n  = 232).

At baseline, emotional eating was significantly associated with perceived stress ( r  = 0.36, p  < .001), barriers to ( r  = 0.31, p  < .001) and motivators of ( r  =  − 0.14, p  < .05) healthy eating, and avoidance coping ( r  = 0.37, p  < .001), but not approach coping. Furthermore, avoidance coping mediated (indirect effect b  = 0.36, 95% CI = 0.13, 0.61) and moderated ( b  =  − 0.07, p  = 0.04) the relationship between perceived stress and emotional eating. Contrary to study hypotheses, baseline stress levels were not associated with emotional eating 1 year later.

College students who utilize avoidance coping strategies may be particularly susceptible to the effects of stress on emotional eating. Healthy eating interventions targeting college students might address stress coping strategies in addition to reduction of barriers to healthy eating.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

i'm stress eating over my dissertation

Similar content being viewed by others

i'm stress eating over my dissertation

Cognitive–behavioral therapy for management of mental health and stress-related disorders: Recent advances in techniques and technologies

Mutsuhiro Nakao, Kentaro Shirotsuki & Nagisa Sugaya

i'm stress eating over my dissertation

Self-Compassion and Bedtime Procrastination: an Emotion Regulation Perspective

Fuschia M. Sirois, Sanne Nauts & Danielle S. Molnar

i'm stress eating over my dissertation

Self-Compassion Interventions and Psychosocial Outcomes: a Meta-Analysis of RCTs

Madeleine Ferrari, Caroline Hunt, … Danielle A. Einstein

Data Availability

The data that support the findings of this study are not openly available due to reasons of sensitivity and are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Martins BG, da Silva WR, Maroco J, Campos JA. Eating behaviors of Brazilian college students: influences of lifestyle, negative affectivity, and personal characteristics. Percept Mot Skills. 2021;128(2):781–99.

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Moss RH, Conner M, O’Connor DB. Exploring the effects of positive and negative emotions on eating behaviours in children and young adults. Psychol Health Med. 2020;26(4):457–66.

Schmalbach I, Herhaus B, Pässler S, Schmalbach B, Berth H, Petrowski K. Effects of stress on chewing and food intake in patients with anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord. 2021;54(7):1160–70.

Hennegan JM, Loxton NJ, Mattar A. Great expectations. Eating expectancies as mediators of reinforcement sensitivity and eating. Appetite. 2013;71:81–88.

Moss RH, Conner M, O’Connor DB. Exploring the effects of daily hassles and uplifts on eating behaviour in young adults: the role of daily cortisol levels. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2021;117.

Wu Y-K, Zimmer C, Munn-Chernoff MA, Baker JH. Association between food addiction and body dissatisfaction among college students: the mediating role of eating expectancies. Eat Behav. 2020;39: 101441.

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Hermes G, Fogelman N, Seo D, Sinha R. Differential effects of recent versus past traumas on mood, social support, binge drinking, emotional eating and BMI, and on neural responses to acute stress. Stress: Int J Biol Stress. 2021;17:1–10.

Ling J, Zahry NR. Relationships among perceived stress, emotional eating, and dietary intake in college students: eating self-regulation as a mediator. Appetite. 2021;163.

Longmire-Avital B, McQueen C. Exploring a relationship between race-related stress and emotional eating for collegiate Black American women. Women Health. 2019;59(3):240–51.

Barnhart WR, Braden AL, Jordan AK. Negative and positive emotional eating uniquely interact with ease of activation, intensity, and duration of emotional reactivity to predict increased binge eating. Appetite. 2020;151: 104688.

Diggins A, Woods-Giscombe C, Waters S. The association of perceived stress, contextualized stress, and emotional eating with body mass index in college-aged Black women. Eat Behav. 2015;19:188–92.

Altheimer G, Urry HL. Do emotions cause eating? The role of previous experiences and social context in emotional eating. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2019;28(3):234–40.

Article   Google Scholar  

Klatzkin RR, Nolan LJ, Kissileff HR. Self-reported emotional eaters consume more food under stress if they experience heightened stress reactivity and emotional relief from stress upon eating. Physiol Behav. 2022;243.

American College Health Association. “National College Health Assessment III: Undergraduate Student Reference Group Executive Summary Spring 2021.” https://www.acha.org/NCHA/ACHA-NCHA_Data/Publications_and_Reports/NCHA/Data/Reports_ACHA-NCHAIIc.aspx . Accessed August 3rd, 2021.

Charles NE, Strong SJ, Burns LC, Bullerjahn MR, Serafine KM. Increased mood disorder symptoms, perceived stress, and alcohol use among college students during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychiatry Res. 2021;296.

Ulrich AK, Full KM, Cheng B, Gravagna K, Nederhoff D, Basta NE. Stress, anxiety, and sleep among college and university students during the COVID-19 pandemic. J Am Coll Health. 2021;1–5.

Krendl AC. Changes in stress predict worse mental health outcomes for college students than does loneliness; evidence from the covid-19 pandemic. J Am Coll Health. 2021;1–4.

Pengpid S, Peltzer K. Vigorous physical activity, perceived stress, sleep and mental health among university students from 23 low- and middle-income countries. Int J Adolesc Med Health. 2020;32(2):1–7.

Google Scholar  

Wu D, Yang T. Late bedtime, uncertainty stress among Chinese college students: impact on academic performance and self-rated health. Psychol Health Med. 2022;1–12.

Koda S, Sugawara K. The influence of diet behavior and stress on binge-eating among female college students. Jpn J Psychol. 2009;80(2):83–9.

Miedema MD, Petrone A, Shikany JM, et al. Association of fruit and vegetable consumption during early adulthood with the prevalence of coronary artery calcium after 20 years of follow-up: The coronary artery risk development in young adults (CARDIA) study. Circulation. 2015;132(21):1990–8.

Article   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Winpenny EM, van Slujis EMF, White M, Klepp K-I, Wold B, Lien N. Changes in diet through adolescence and early adulthood: longitudinal trajectories and association with key life transitions. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2018;15.

LaCaille LJ, Dauner KN, Krambeer RJ, Pedersen J. Psychosocial and environmental determinants of eating behaviors, physical activity, and weight change among college students: A qualitative analysis. J Am Coll Health. 2011;59(6):531–8.

Poobalan AS, Aucot LS, Clarke A, Smith WC. Diet behaviour among young people in transition to adulthood (18–25 year olds): a mixed method study. Health Psychol Behav Med. 2014;2(1):909–28.

Sogari G, Velez-Argumedo C, Gómez MI, Mora C. College students and eating habits: a study using an ecological model for healthy behavior. Nutrients. 2018;10(12):1823.

Hilger J, Loerbroks A, Diehl K. Eating behaviour of university students in Germany: dietary intake, barriers to healthy eating and changes in eating behaviour since the time of matriculation. Appetite. 2017;109:100–7.

Aljasem LI, Peyrot M, Wissow L, Rubin RR. The impact of barriers and self efficacy on self-care behaviors in type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Educ. 2001;27(3):393–404.

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Fitzgerald A, Heary C, Kelly C, Nixon E, Shevlin M. Self-efficacy for healthy eating and peer support for unhealthy eating are associated with adolescents’ food intake patterns. Appetite. 2013;63(1):48–58.

Zajacova A, Lynch SM, Espenshad TJ. Self-efficacy, stress, and academic success in college. Res High Educ. 2005;46:677–706.

Moos RH. Coping responses inventory: a measure of approach and avoidance coping skills. In: Zalaquett CP, Wood RJ, editors. Evaluating stress: a book of resources. University of Michigan: Scarecrow Press; 1997. p. 51–65.

Gustems-Carnicer J, Calderón C. Virtues and character strengths related to approach coping strategies of college students. Social Psychol Educ. 2016;19(1):77–95.

Moos RH. Coping responses inventory: adult form: professional manual. Odessa: PAR Inc.; 1993.

Konaszewski K, Kolemba M, Niesiobędzka M. Resilience, sense of coherence and self-efficacy as predictors of stress coping style among university students. Curr Psychol. 2021;40(8):4052–62.

Spoor STP, Bekker MHJ, Van Strien T, van Heck GL. Relations between negative affect, coping, and emotional eating. Appetite. 2007;48(3):368–76.

Yönder Ertem M, Karakaş M. Relationship between emotional eating and coping with stress of nursing students. Perspect Psychiatr Care. 2020;57(2):433–42.

Young D, Limbers CA. Avoidant coping moderates the relationship between stress and depressive emotional eating in adolescents. Eat Weight Disord. 2017;22(4):683–91.

Errisuriz VL, Pasch KE, Perry CL. Perceived stress and dietary choices: the moderating role of stress management. Eat Behav. 2016;22:211–6.

van den Tol AJM, Ward MR, Fong H. The role of coping in emotional eating and the use of music for discharge when feeling stressed. Arts Psychother. 2019;64:95–103.

Xie A, Cai TS, He JB, Liu W-L, Liu J-X. Effects of negative emotion on emotional eating of college students: mediating role of negative coping style. Chin J Clin Psychol. 2016;24(2):298–301.

United States Department of Agriculture. MyPlate. 2018.  https://www.myplate.gov/

Stadler G, Oettingen G, Gollwitzer PM. Intervention effects of information and self-regulation on eating fruits and vegetables over two years. Health Psychol. 2010;29(3):274–83.

Ozier AD, Kendrick OW, Knol LL, Leeper JD, Perko M, Burnham JJ. Development and validation: the EADES (eating and appraisal due to emotions and stress) questionnaire. J Am Diet Assoc. 2007;107(4):619–28.

Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Social Behav. 1983;24:386–96.

Lee E-H. Review of the psychometric evidence of the perceived stress scale. Asian Nurs Res. 2012;6(4):121–7.

Tucker CM, Rice KG, Hou W, et al. development of the motivators of and barriers to health-smart behaviors inventory. Psychol Assess. 2011;23(2):487–503.

Carver CS. You want to measure coping but your protocol’s too long: consider the BriefCOPE. Int J Behav Med. 1997;4:92–100.

Eisenberg SA, Shen BJ, Schwarz ER, Mallon S. Avoidant coping moderates the association between anxiety and patient-rated physical functioning in heart failure patients. J Behav Med. 2012;35(3):253–61.

Wichianson JR, Bughu SA, Unger JB, Sprujit-Metz D, Nguyen-Rodriguez ST. Perceive stress, coping, and night-eating in college students. Stress Health. 2009;25(3):235–40.

Byrne BM. Structural equation modeling with AMOS: basic concepts, applications, and programming. Routledge. 2010.

Hair J, Black WC, Babin BJ, Anderson RE. Multivariate data analysis , 7th ed. London, UK: Pearson Educational International. 2010.

Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a conditional process analysis. 3rd ed. New York, NY: The Guilford Press; 2022.

Hayes AF, Montoya AK, Rockwood NJ. The analysis of mechanisms and their contingencies: PROCESS versus structural equation modeling. Australas Mark J. 2017;25:76–81.

Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc. 1995;57(1):289–300.

Brosof LC, Munn-Chernoff MA, Bulik CM, Baker JH. Associations between eating expectancies and eating disorder symptoms in men and women. Appetite. 2019;141.

Hayaki J, Free S. Positive and negative eating expectancies in disordered eating among women and men. Eat Behav. 2016;22:22–6.

Smith JM, Serier KN, Belon KE, Sebastian RM, Smith JE. Evaluation of the relationships between dietary restraint, emotional eating, and intuitive eating moderated by sex. Appetite. 2020;155(1): 104817.

Greene D, Mullins M, Baggett P, Cherry D. Self-care for helping professionals: students’ perceived stress, coping self- efficacy, and subjective experiences. J Bacc Soc Work. 2017;22:1–16.

Ling J, Zahry NR. Relationships among perceived stress, emotional eating, and dietary intake in college students: Eating self-regulation as a mediator. Appetite. 2021;163.

Marshall SJ, Elliott CC. Predicting college students’ food intake quality with dimensions of executive functioning. J Appl Biobehav Res. 2016;237- 252.

Pribis P, Burtnack CA, McKenzie SO, Thayer J. Trends in body fat, body mass index and physical fitness among male and female college students. Nutrients. 2010;2(10):1075–85.

Quintiliani LM, Whiteley JA. Results of a nutrition and physical activity peer counseling intervention among nontraditional college students. J Cancer Educ. 2016;31:366–74.

Schroeter C, House LA. Fruit and vegetable consumption of college students: what is the role of food culture? J Food Distrib Res. 2015;46(3):131–52.

Son C, Hegde S, Smith A, Wang X, Sasangohar F. Effects of COVID-19 on college students’ mental health in the United States: interview survey study. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22(9): e21279.

Peltzer K, Pengpid S. Dietary consumption and happiness and depression among university students: A cross-national survey. J Psychol Afr. 2017;27(4):372–7.

Wright RR, Shuai J, Maldonado Y, Nelson C. The cents program: promoting healthy eating by addressing perceived barriers. Psychol Health. 2021;1:1–19.

Brown ON, O’Connor LE, Savaiano D. Mobile MyPlate: a pilot study using text messaging to provide nutrition education and promote better dietary choices in college students. J Am Coll Health. 2014;62(5):320–7.

Bernardo GL, Rodrigues VM, Bastos BS, et al. Association of personal characteristics and cooking skills with vegetable consumption frequency among university students. Appetite. 2021;166.

Martin S, McCormack L. Eating behaviors and the perceived nutrition environment among college students. J Am Coll Health. 2022.

Vidic Z. Multi-year investigation of a relaxation course with a mindfulness meditation component on college students’ stress, resilience, coping and mindfulness. J Am Coll Health. 2021;1–6.

Yusufov M, Nicoloro-Santa BJ, Grey NE, Moyer A, Lobel M. Meta- analytic evaluation of stress reduction interventions for undergraduate and graduate students. Int J Stress Manag. 2019;26(2):132–45.

Download references

Acknowledgements

The author wishes to thank Yusuf Chaudhry, Elizabeth Doll, Nakita Edwards, Cheryl Errichetti, Mackenzie Graf, Sarah Kaden, Madison Lasko, and Alexandra Palumbo for their assistance with data collection.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Psychology, Elizabethtown College, 1 Alpha Drive, Elizabethtown, PA, 17022, USA

Elizabeth D. Dalton

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elizabeth D. Dalton .

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval.

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants in the study.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Dalton, E.D. Emotional Eating in College Students: Associations with Coping and Healthy Eating Motivators and Barriers. Int.J. Behav. Med. (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-023-10193-y

Download citation

Accepted : 06 June 2023

Published : 29 June 2023

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-023-10193-y

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Emotional eating
  • College students
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • HHS Author Manuscripts

Logo of nihpa

Stress Eating and Health: Findings from MIDUS, a National Study of U.S. Adults

The epidemic of obesity and its related chronic diseases has provoked interest in the predictors of eating behavior. Eating in response to stress has been extensively examined, but currently unclear is whether stress eating is associated with obesity and morbidity. We tested whether self-reported stress eating was associated with worse glucose metabolism among nondiabetic adults as well as with increased odds of prediabetes and diabetes. Further, we investigated whether these relationships were mediated by central fat distribution. Participants were 1138 adults (937 without diabetes) in the Midlife in the U.S. study (MIDUS II). Glucose metabolism was characterized by fasting glucose, insulin, insulin resistance (HOMAIR), glycosylated hemoglobin (HbA 1c ), prediabetes, and diabetes status. Multivariate-adjusted analyses showed that stress eating was associated with significantly higher nondiabetic levels of glucose, insulin, insulin resistance, and HbA 1c as well as higher odds of prediabetes or diabetes. Relationships between stress eating and all outcomes were no longer statistically significant once waist circumference was added to the models, suggesting that it mediates such relationships. Findings add to the growing literature on the relationships among psychosocial factors, obesity, and chronic disease by documenting associations between stress eating and objectively measured health outcomes in a national sample of adults. The findings have important implications for interventive targets related to obesity and chronic disease, namely, strategies to modify the tendency to use food as a coping response to stress.

Introduction

Obesity is the most critical factor in the development of metabolic disease, with contemporary environments frequently described as obesogenic, or promoting obesity via abundant availability of energy-dense food accompanied by decline in physical activity ( Chaput, Klingenberg, Astrup, & Sjodin, 2011 ; Swinburn, Egger, & Raza, 1999 ). More than 1/3 of American adults were obese in 2009–2010 ( Ogden, Carroll, Kit, & Flegal, 2012 ); 25.8 million Americans have diabetes and 79 million have prediabetes ( Centers for Disease Control, 2011 ). Traditionally, research has focused on diet and physical activity as cornerstones of obesity prevention and treatment, although it is clear that these factors leave considerable variance unexplained. Investigators have thus concentrated on identifying factors that extend the concept of energy balance by describing pathways to energy imbalance. Studies on triggers of eating behaviors document that a negative energy balance is a sufficient, but not necessary condition for initiating an eating episode ( Del Parigi, 2010 ). What matters as well for initiation of an eating episode are complex interactions among emotional, cognitive, and cultural factors ( Del Parigi, 2010 ).

Eating in excess of metabolic needs is, in fact, the leading contributor to weight gain, obesity, and subsequent morbidity. The link between stress and eating has received significant attention, given the considerable overlap between the physiological systems that regulate food intake and that mediate the stress response ( Tannenbaum, 2010 ). During stress, the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis is activated to prepare the organism for fight or fight; that is, mounting a defensive response depends on available energy. Thus, the HPA axis initiates a cascade of physiological adaptations such as the release of glucose into the bloodstream, thereby suppressing hunger ( Gold & Chrousos, 2002 ). Emotional eaters, however, do not show the typical response of eating less during stress ( Gold & Chrousos, 2002 ). Instead, they eat the same amount, or more, during stress ( Oliver, Wardle, & Gibson, 2000 ; van Strien & Ouwens, 2003 ). It has been suggested that people use “comfort food”, meaning food high in sugar and fat, in an effort to reduce activity in the chronic stress-response network with its attendant anxiety. Intake of comfort food is thought to alleviate stress by reducing HPA axis activity and promoting the activation of brain circuits involved in reward-seeking behavior ( Dallman et al., 2003 ), thereby further reinforcing feeding behavior ( Dallman, 2010 ).

Self-reported stress levels have been increasing over time ( Cohen, 2012 ). In addition, acute and chronic stressors have been linked to energy-dense food intake, weight gain, obesity and glucoregulation ( Bjorntorp, 2001 ; Block, He, Zaslavsky, Ding, & Ayanian, 2009 ; Eriksson et al., 2008 ; Groesz et al., 2012 ; Heraclides, Chandola, Witte, & Brunner, 2009 ; Laitinen, Ek, & Sovio, 2002 ; Ng & Jeffery, 2003 ). A recent national survey documented that 39% of people overeat, or increase consumption of energy-dense foods in response to stress ( American Psychological Association, 2012 ). Nonetheless, it remains unclear whether eating in response to stress has metabolic consequences such as dysregulated glycemic control and abdominal obesity. One study looked at the relationship between self-reported stress eating and metabolic syndrome in medical students and found that stress eaters showed significant increases in weight and insulin during an exam period compared to students who reported eating less during stress ( Epel et al., 2004 ).

To our knowledge, no study has examined relationships among stress eating, obesity, and metabolic disease in a national sample. The overarching goal of this investigation was to assess whether eating in response to stress was associated with glucoregulation. Further, we evaluated the role of waist circumference as a potential mediator of the relationship between stress eating and glucoregulation. The underlying rationales were first, that stress activation has been implicated in the pathogenesis of abdominal obesity ( Bjorntorp, 2001 ; Dallman, Pecoraro, & la Fleur, 2005 ), and second, that waist circumference is a powerful predictor of diabetes ( Klein et al., 2007 ). The specific hypotheses of our study therefore were:

  • Hypothesis 1 (H1). (a) Eating in response to stress will be associated with worse nondiabetic levels of fasting glucose, insulin, insulin resistance, and HbA 1c . (b) Waist circumference will mediate the relationships between using food in response to stress and fasting glucose, insulin, insulin resistance, and HbA 1c
  • Hypothesis 2 (H2). (a) Extending the prior hypotheses to disease progression, eating in response to stress will be associated with higher odds of prediabetes in the nondiabetic subsample as well as diabetes in the full analytical sample. (b) Waist circumference will mediate the relationships between using food in response to stress and higher odds of prediabetes in the nondiabetic subsample and diabetes in the full analytical sample.

Material and Methods

Data are from the Midlife in the US II (MIDUS II) study, a longitudinal follow-up of the original MIDUS sample (N = 7,108). Begun in 1995/96, the overarching objective of MIDUS was to investigate the role of behavioral, psychological, and social factors in physical and mental health. All eligible participants were non-institutionalized, English-speaking adults in the coterminous United States, initially 25 to 74 years of age. Approximately 9–10 years later, respondents were re-contacted and invited to participate in MIDUS II. The longitudinal retention rate was 75%, adjusted for mortality (Radler & Ryff, 2010). One objective of MIDUS II was to extend the scientific scope of the study by adding comprehensive biological assessments on a subsample of respondents who had completed a phone interview and self-administered questionnaires. Forty-three percent of the invited MIDUS II respondents participated in the biological data collection. The majority (87.8%) of African American respondents came from a city-specific sample from Milwaukee, Wisconsin, which was implemented to increase participation of African Americans in the biological data collection, given its close proximity to one of the clinic sites. This full biomarker sample was not significantly different from the main MIDUS sample on age, sex, race, marital status, or income variables, although participants were significantly more educated than the main sample ( Love, Seeman, Weinstein, & Ryff, 2010 ).

The current analyses used data from the biological sample of MIDUS II and included 1255 participants ages 34 to 84 ( M =54.52, SD =11.71), more than half of whom (57%) were female. Only respondents who identified as White or African American were included in the current study because small sample sizes precluded the inclusion of other minority groups. After excluding 117 cases due to partially missing data on any variable in the analysis, or to a race other than black or white, 1138 participants had complete data. Table 1 includes descriptive information for all variables in the analyses.

Means (and SDs) or Proportions for All Measures Stratified by Stress Eating 1 and Diabetes Status

Note: High stress eating group consists of people who answered the questions the two questions about food quantity and preference with “a lot” or “a medium amount.” Low stress eating group consists of people who answered the questions with “only a little” and “not at all.” Stress eating was dichotomized into high and low stress eating groups for descriptive analyses only; a continuous measure of stress eating was used in all regression analyses.

Glucoregulation was indexed using three primary biological measures (fasting glucose, fasting insulin, and HbA 1c ) and three composite measures (insulin resistance, prediabetes, diabetes). Fasting glucose, insulin, and HbA 1c samples were obtained during an overnight stay in a General Clinical Research Center (GCRC). Fasting glucose was measured via an enzymatic assay photometrically on an automated analyzer (Roche Modular Analytics P). Fasting insulin was measured with an ADVIA Centaur Insulin assay, performed on a Siemens Advia Centaur analyzer. The HbA 1c assay was a colorimetric total-hemoglobin determination combined with an immunoturbidometric HbA 1c assay, carried out using a Cobas Integra Systems instrument (Roche Diagnostics) ( Wolf, Lang, & Zander, 1984 ). The first composite measure (insulin resistance) was calculated using the Homeostasis Model Assessment (HOMA-IR) formula that incorporates both glucose and insulin to describe the interplay between them ( Matthews et al., 1985 ).

The other composites were dichotomous categorizations that used criteria from the American Diabetes Association to define presence of prediabetes or diabetes ( 2013 ). Specifically, nondiabetic individuals were classified as “prediabetic” if their HbA 1c was between 5.7–6.5% or their glucose was between 100–126 mg/dl. In the full analytical sample, diabetes status was coded positive if HbA 1c exceeded 6.5%, fasting glucose exceeded 126 mg/dl, or participant reported taking anti-diabetic medications. Using these criteria, 201 were coded as diabetic. All primary and composite measures are reliable and widely used in clinical practice to predict risk for disease ( Ausk, Boyko, & Ioannou, 2010 ; Balkau et al., 1998 ; Bloomgarden, 2011a , 2011b ; Parekh, Lin, Hayes, Albu, & Lu-Yao, 2010 ; Skriver, Borch-Johnsen, Lauritzen, & Sandbaek, 2010 ). Central fat distribution was indexed by waist circumference (WC), measured by a GCRC staff member around the abdomen just above the hip bone.

Respondents were asked to indicate how they “usually experience a stressful event,” two options of which were “I eat more of my favorite foods to make myself feel better” and “I eat more than I usually do.” Responses ranged from 1= a lot to 4= not at all . Responses to the two items were reverse coded and summed so that higher scores indicated greater use of food in response to stress. The correlation between the two items was .81 in the nondiabetic subsample and .80 in the full analytical sample.

All models were multivariate-adjusted for relevant covariates. Age, household income, and education (12 categories ranged from no school to completion of a professional degree) were treated as continuous variables. Race (black or white) and gender (male or female) were categorical variables.

Data Analysis

Hierarchical multiple regression models were used to predict waist circumference and glycemic control indices (fasting glucose, insulin, insulin resistance, and HbA 1c ). Fasting insulin and HOMA-IR were log-transformed to achieve normal distributions. Binary logistic regression models were used to predict prediabetes and diabetes status. Age and income were rescaled in the regression models such that one unit in the age variable was equal to 10 years and one unit in the income variable was equal to 10,000 dollars. We employed Baron and Kenny’s (1986) criteria for testing waist circumference as a mediator. Additionally, we estimated percent explained (PE), a summary measure of the indirect effect that clarifies the extent to which the effect of stress eating on the different indicators of glucose metabolism was mediated by WC.

Preliminary analyses tested for gender, age, and race differences in the associations between using food to cope and glucoregulation by estimating a model with three two-way interaction terms created by multiplying combinations of gender, age, and race by stress eating. Since none of the interactions was significant, the entire sample was analyzed as one group. All models included all covariates (age, gender, race, education, and household income).

Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1 .

H1: Stress eating will predict higher levels of glucose, insulin, insulin resistance, and HbA 1c and these effects will be mediated by waist circumference

Model 1 (see Table 2 ) displays estimates with respect to H1. Consistent with H1, multivariate-adjusted models confirmed that stress eating was associated with higher glucose ( b =.67, p <.001), log-transformed insulin ( b =.08, p <.001), log-transformed insulin resistance ( b =.08, p <.001), and HbA 1c ( b =.02, p <.01). Stress eating was linked with waist circumference in the nondiabetic subsample ( b =.91, p <.001) and waist circumference was associated with all measures of glucoregulation ( p <.001 for all) in multivariate-adjusted models. In contrast to Model 1 estimates, coefficients in Model 1.1—which added waist circumference—indicated that the associations between stress eating and glucose, log-transformed insulin, log-transformed insulin resistance, and HbA 1c were reduced in size and no longer significant ( p ranged from .1 to .2), thus demonstrating that WC mediated the relationships between stress eating and all measures of glucoregulation. Using PE, we found that waist circumference mediated a significant part of the effect of stress eating on glucose (61% PE), log-transformed insulin (81% PE), log-transformed insulin resistance (79% PE), and HbA 1c (56% PE).

Linear Regression Results for Stress Eating, Waist Circumference, and Nondiabetic Glucoregulation (N=937)

Note: Unstandardized coefficients are shown. Insulin and insulin resistance are log-transformed. The age and income variables have been rescaled: one unit in the rescaled age variable is equal to 10 years and one unit in the rescaled income variable is equal to 10,000 dollars.

H2: Stress eating will predict prediabetes and diabetes and these effects will be mediated by waist circumference

Model 2 (see Table 3 ) displays results for the logistic regression models documenting that stress eating was associated with prediabetes ( OR =1.09, p <.05) in the nondiabetic sample and diabetes ( OR =1.15, p <.002) in the full analytical sample. Stress eating was linked with waist circumference in the full sample ( β =.93, p <.001) and waist circumference was associated with all measures of glucoregulation ( p <.001 for all). In contrast to Model 2 estimates, once waist circumference was added, stress eating was no longer a significant predictor of prediabetes ( p =.7) or diabetes status ( p =.3) (See Model 2.1), thus providing supportive evidence that higher waist circumference mediates the relationship between stress eating and glucoregulation.

Estimated odds ratios for the associations between stress eating, prediabetes, and diabetes.

Note: The age and income variables have been rescaled: one unit in the rescaled age variable is equal to 10 years and one unit in the rescaled income variable is equal to 10,000 dollars.

Discussion and Conclusions

The objective of this study was to examine relationships between stress eating and clinically significant measures of metabolic health. The “flight or fight” response to stress causes release of glucose in the bloodstream, which is known to suppress appetite. However, for many people, the response to stressful situations is not to avoid eating, but to consume high volumes of energy-dense comfort foods. Using continuous measures of fasting glucose, insulin, insulin resistance, and HbA 1c , we documented that stress eating was associated with worse glycemic control and prediabetes status among nondiabetic adults as well as diabetes in the full sample. Importantly, waist circumference was found to mediate these relationships. Taken together, these results confirm and extend results from a smaller experimental study ( Epel et al., 2004 ), and suggest that stress eating is associated with objectively measured glucoregulation outcomes and specific diabetic morbidities, with the effects occurring primarily through central adiposity.

Traditional weight loss interventions often focus on diet and exercise, and although they may result in temporary weight loss, recurrent weight gain is common. A meta-analysis of weight loss interventions documented that 5 years post intervention three-fourths of the lost weight was regained ( Anderson, Konz, Frederich, & Wood, 2001 ). One reason for relapse may be that traditional interventions do not target the root cause of overeating and inactivity. Using food to cope with stress may be one such underlying cause that is a potential target of intervention, as the response may be modifiable. If so, it could offer an important step toward incorporating characteristics of the individual in obesity treatment programs (National Institutes of Health, 1998 ). Stressors are thought to activate a neural stress-response network that promotes emotional activity and degrades executive function, resulting in employment of formed habits rather than cognitive resources ( Dallman, 2010 ). Coping with stress by eating palatable foods may thus reduce anxiety and perceived stress, while further reinforcing the feeding habit. To reduce such stress-induced eating, it is important to identify alternative strategies that promote cognitive, goal-directed responses to stress. For example, little is known about the relationships between coping with food and problem- and emotion-focused coping skills ( Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989 ). If comfort feeding is the default response and no active behavioral or emotional strategies are available to deal with the stressor, it is easy to understand how stressors and stress eating reinforce each other, with consequences for promoting obesity and chronic disease.

Mindfulness-based training ( Kristeller & Wolever, 2011 ) may be relevant, given the focus on guided practices to address responses to different emotional states as well as awareness of hunger and satiety cues and related links to conscious food choices. Emerging findings from mindfulness-based interventions document improvements in weight, eating habits, and mental health ( Alberts, Mulkens, Smeets, & Thewissen, 2010 ; Alberts, Thewissen, & Raes, 2012 ; Dalen et al., 2010 ; Daubenmier et al., 2011 ). Importantly, empowering people to cultivate awareness of emotional triggers and eating patterns may also promote self-acceptance ( Kristeller & Wolever, 2011 ), which converges with the idea that effective motivational strategies for health promotion must focus on adopting health behaviors without referencing body weight ( Puhl, Peterson, & Luedicke, 2012 ).

Several limitations of the present study need to be acknowledged. The absence of longitudinal data on stress eating and glucose metabolism limits the strength of the conclusions regarding the directionality of described relationships. However, previous prospective research linking stress eating to metabolic dysregulation ( Epel et al., 2004 ) makes it less likely that poor glycemic control was the precipitating event that promoted stress eating. Another limitation is that many of the African-American respondents were drawn from a city-specific oversample (i.e., Milwaukee) implemented to increase participation of Blacks in the MIDUS biomarker project. Thus, they are not nationally representative. It should be noted, however, that the findings were not driven by racial factors, nor were there differences in the pattern of associations between white and black participants. It is not clear if the results generalize to other ethnicities. Our analyses also did not examine the varieties of stress that may be precursors to stress eating, for which MIDUS has multiple indicators (e.g., job stress, caregiving stress, daily stress, perceived discrimination, work/family conflict). Other individual difference variables, such as personality characteristics (e.g., neuroticism) may add further precision in identifying those most susceptible to stress eating. Finally, our analyses are modeled to capture influences on type 2 diabetes, but we did not have information on whether participants in the diabetes category had type 1 or type 2 diabetes. Given that approximately 90–95% of people with diabetes have type 2 diabetes ( American Diabetes Association, 2013 ), our results are not significantly affected by this imprecision. Despite these caveats, the findings that stress eating was associated with increased waist circumference and worse glucoregulation helps advance understanding of the psychosocial underpinnings of obesity and glucoregulation. Continuing to elucidate the various processes that underlie energy imbalance and behavioral responses to it is critical for developing effective preventive and interventive efforts related to obesity and its disease sequelae.

  • We investigate whether stress eating predicts health in a national sample of adults.
  • Specific outcomes include nondiabetic glycemic control, pre-diabetes, and diabetes.
  • We find that stress eating predicts these outcomes.
  • Waist circumference mediates the relationships between stress eating and health.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a grant from the National Institute on Aging (P01-AG020166; Carol D. Ryff, Principal Investigator) to conduct a longitudinal follow-up of the MIDUS (Midlife in the US) investigation. The original study was supported by the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Successful Midlife Development. We thank the staff of the Clinical Research Center at the University of Wisconsin-Madison, at the University of California—Los Angeles, and at Georgetown University for their support in conducting this study. Data collection was supported by the following grants M01-RR023942 (Georgetown), M01-RR00865 (UCLA) from the General Clinical Research Centers Program, and 1UL1RR025011 (UW) from the Clinical and Translational Science Award (CTSA) program of the National Center for Research Resources, National Institutes of Health. The first author of this study was supported by the National Institute on Aging (K01AG041179) and by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health And Human Development (T32HD049302). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development or the National Institutes of Health.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

  • Alberts HJ, Mulkens S, Smeets M, Thewissen R. Coping with food cravings. Investigating the potential of a mindfulness-based intervention. Appetite. 2010; 55 (1):160–163. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Alberts HJ, Thewissen R, Raes L. Dealing with problematic eating behaviour. The effects of a mindfulness-based intervention on eating behaviour, food cravings, dichotomous thinking and body image concern. Appetite. 2012; 290:58 (3):847–851. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • American Diabetes Association. Diagnosis and classification of diabetes mellitus. Diabetes Care. 2013; 36 (Suppl 1):S67–74. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • American Psychological Association. Stress in America: Our Health at Risk [Press Release] 2012. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Anderson JW, Konz EC, Frederich RC, Wood CL. Long-term weight-loss maintenance: a meta-analysis of US studies. Am J Clin Nutr. 2001; 74 (5):579–584. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ausk KJ, Boyko EJ, Ioannou GN. Insulin resistance predicts mortality in nondiabetic individuals in the U.S. Diabetes Care. 2010; 33 (6):1179–1185. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Balkau B, Shipley M, Jarrett RJ, Pyorala K, Pyorala M, Forhan A, Eschwege E. High blood glucose concentration is a risk factor for mortality in middle-aged nondiabetic men. 20-year follow-up in the Whitehall Study, the Paris Prospective Study, and the Helsinki Policemen Study. Diabetes Care. 1998; 21 (3):360–367. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986; 51 (6):1173–1182. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bjorntorp P. Do stress reactions cause abdominal obesity and comorbidities? Obes Rev. 2001; 2 (2):73–86. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Block JP, He Y, Zaslavsky AM, Ding L, Ayanian JZ. Psychosocial stress and change in weight among US adults. Am J Epidemiol. 2009; 170 (2):181–192. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bloomgarden ZT. World congress on insulin resistance, diabetes, and cardiovascular disease: Part 1. Diabetes Care. 2011a; 34 (7):e115–120. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bloomgarden ZT. World congress on insulin resistance, diabetes, and cardiovascular disease: part 2. Diabetes Care. 2011b; 34 (8):e126–131. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carver CS, Scheier MF, Weintraub JK. Assessing coping strategies: a theoretically based approach. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989; 56 (2):267–283. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Centers for Disasea Control and Prevention. National diabetes fact sheet: national estimates and general information on diabetes and prediabetes in the United States, 2011. Atlanta, GA: 2011. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chaput JP, Klingenberg L, Astrup A, Sjodin AM. Modern sedentary activities promote overconsumption of food in our current obesogenic environment. Obes Rev. 2011; 12 (5):e12–20. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D. Who’s Stressed? Distributions of Psychological Stress in the United States in Probability Samples from 1983, 2006, and 2009. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2012; 42 :1320–1334. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dalen J, Smith BW, Shelley BM, Sloan AL, Leahigh L, Begay D. Pilot study: Mindful Eating and Living (MEAL): weight, eating behavior, and psychological outcomes associated with a mindfulness-based intervention for people with obesity. Complement Ther Med. 2010; 18 (6):260–264. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dallman MF. Stress-induced obesity and the emotional nervous system. Trends Endocrinol Metab. 2010; 21 (3):159–165. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dallman MF, Pecoraro N, Akana SF, La Fleur SE, Gomez F, Houshyar H, Manalo S. Chronic stress and obesity: a new view of “comfort food” Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003; 100 (20):11696–11701. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dallman MF, Pecoraro NC, la Fleur SE. Chronic stress and comfort foods: self-medication and abdominal obesity. Brain Behav Immun. 2005; 19 (4):275–280. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Daubenmier J, Kristeller J, Hecht FM, Maninger N, Kuwata M, Jhaveri K, Epel E. Mindfulness Intervention for Stress Eating to Reduce Cortisol and Abdominal Fat among Overweight and Obese Women: An Exploratory Randomized Controlled Study. J Obes. 2011; 2011 :651936. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Del Parigi A. Neuroanatomical correlates of hunger and satiety in lean and obese individuals. In: Dube L, Bechara A, Dagher A, Drewnowski A, LeBel J, James P, Yada R, editors. Obesity Prevention. 1. Academic Press; 2010. pp. 253–271. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Epel E, Jimenez S, Brownell K, Stroud L, Stoney C, Niaura R. Are stress eaters at risk for the metabolic syndrome? Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004; 1032 :208–210. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eriksson AK, Ekbom A, Granath F, Hilding A, Efendic S, Ostenson CG. Psychological distress and risk of pre-diabetes and Type 2 diabetes in a prospective study of Swedish middle-aged men and women. Diabet Med. 2008; 25 (7):834–842. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gold PW, Chrousos GP. Organization of the stress system and its dysregulation in melancholic and atypical depression: high vs low CRH/NE states. Mol Psychiatry. 2002; 7 (3):254–275. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Groesz LM, McCoy S, Carl J, Saslow L, Stewart J, Adler N, Epel E. What is eating you? Stress and the drive to eat. Appetite. 2012; 58 (2):717–721. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • NIo Health. Clinical Guidelines on the Identification, Evaluation, and Treatment of Overweight and Obesity in Adults--The Evidence Report. National Institutes of Health. Obes Res. 1998; 6 (Suppl 2):51S–209S. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Heraclides A, Chandola T, Witte DR, Brunner EJ. Psychosocial stress at work doubles the risk of type 2 diabetes in middle-aged women: evidence from the Whitehall II study. Diabetes Care. 2009; 32 (12):2230–2235. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Klein S, Allison DB, Heymsfield SB, Kelley DE, Leibel RL, Nonas C, Kahn R. Waist Circumference and Cardiometabolic Risk: a Consensus Statement from Shaping America’s Health: Association for Weight Management and Obesity Prevention; NAASO, the Obesity Society; the American Society for Nutrition; and the American Diabetes Association. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2007; 15 (5):1061–1067. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kristeller JL, Wolever RQ. Mindfulness-based eating awareness training for treating binge eating disorder: the conceptual foundation. Eat Disord. 2011; 19 (1):49–61. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Laitinen J, Ek E, Sovio U. Stress-related eating and drinking behavior and body mass index and predictors of this behavior. Prev Med. 2002; 34 (1):29–39. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Love GD, Seeman TE, Weinstein M, Ryff CD. Bioindicators in the MIDUS National Study: Protocol, Measures, Sample, and Comparative Context. J Aging Health. 2010; 22 (8):1059–1080. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Matthews DR, Hosker JP, Rudenski AS, Naylor BA, Treacher DF, Turner RC. Homeostasis model assessment: insulin resistance and beta-cell function from fasting plasma glucose and insulin concentrations in man. Diabetologia. 1985; 28 (7):412–419. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ng DM, Jeffery RW. Relationships between perceived stress and health behaviors in a sample of working adults. Health Psychol. 2003; 22 (6):638–642. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Kit BK, Flegal KM. Prevalence of obesity in the United States, 2009–2010. NCHS Data Brief. 2012;(82):1–8. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Oliver G, Wardle J, Gibson EL. Stress and food choice: a laboratory study. Psychosom Med. 2000; 62 (6):853–865. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Parekh N, Lin Y, Hayes RB, Albu JB, Lu-Yao GL. Longitudinal associations of blood markers of insulin and glucose metabolism and cancer mortality in the third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Cancer Causes Control. 2010; 21 (4):631–642. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Puhl R, Peterson JL, Luedicke J. Fighting obesity or obese persons? Public perceptions of obesity-related health messages. Int J Obes (Lond) 2012 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Skriver MV, Borch-Johnsen K, Lauritzen T, Sandbaek A. HbA1c as predictor of all-cause mortality in individuals at high risk of diabetes with normal glucose tolerance, identified by screening: a follow-up study of the Anglo-Danish-Dutch Study of Intensive Treatment in People with Screen-Detected Diabetes in Primary Care (ADDITION), Denmark. Diabetologia. 2010; 53 (11):2328–2333. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Swinburn B, Egger G, Raza F. Dissecting obesogenic environments: the development and application of a framework for identifying and prioritizing environmental interventions for obesity. Prev Med. 1999; 29 (6 Pt 1):563–570. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tannenbaum B, Anisman H, Abizaid A. Neuroendocrine stress response and its impact on eating behavior and body weight. In: Dube L, Bechara A, Dagher A, Drewnowski A, LeBel J, James P, Yada R, editors. Obesity Prevention. Academic Press; 2010. [ Google Scholar ]
  • van Strien T, Ouwens MA. Counterregulation in female obese emotional eaters: Schachter, Goldman, and Gordon’s (1968) test of psychosomatic theory revisited. Eat Behav. 2003; 3 (4):329–340. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wolf HU, Lang W, Zander R. Alkaline haematin D-575, a new tool for the determination of haemoglobin as an alternative to the cyanhaemiglobin method. II. Standardisation of the method using pure chlorohaemin. Clin Chim Acta. 1984; 136 :1–104. 0009-8981(84)90251-1 [pii] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Stress and eating behavior: a daily diary study in youngsters.

\r\nTaaike Debeuf*

  • 1 Department of Developmental, Personality and Social Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
  • 2 Department of Public Health, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium

Background: Overweight and obesity are growing problems, with more attention recently, to the role of stress in the starting and maintaining process of these clinical problems. However, the mechanisms are not yet known and well-understood; and ecological momentary analyses like the daily variations between stress and eating are far less studied. Emotional eating is highly prevalent and is assumed to be an important mechanism, as a maladaptive emotion regulation (ER) strategy, in starting and maintaining the vicious cycle of (pediatric) obesity.

Objectives: The present study aims to investigate in youngsters (10 – 17 years) the daily relationship between stress and the trajectories of self-reported eating behavior (desire to eat motives; hunger eating motives and snacking) throughout 1 week; as well as the moderating role of emotion regulation and emotional eating in an average weight population.

Methods: Participants were 109 average weighted youngsters between the age of 10 and 17 years ( M age = 13.49; SD = 1.64). The youngsters filled in a trait-questionnaire on emotion regulation and emotional eating at home before starting the study, and answered an online diary after school time, during seven consecutive days. Desire to eat motives, hunger eating motives and snacking were assessed daily for seven consecutive days.

Results: Using multilevel analyses results revealed that daily stress is significantly associated with trajectories of desire to eat motives and hunger eating motives. No evidence was found for the moderating role of maladaptive ER in these relationships; marginally significant evidence was found for the moderating role of emotional eating in the trajectories of desire to eat and snacking.

Discussion: These results stress the importance of looking into the daily relationship between stress and eating behavior parameters, as both are related with change over and within days. More research is needed to draw firm conclusion on the moderating role of ER strategies and emotional eating.

Introduction

Pediatric overweight and obesity are growing problems, as the prevalence of obesity is tripled since 1975. Worldwide, 18% of the children and adolescents (5–19 years) are diagnosed with overweight and 8% with obesity ( World Health Organization [WHO], 2018 ). Parallel to the global rise in pediatric obesity, metabolic, psychological and social health problems are beginning to appear in childhood; and positive associations between these phenomena are found ( Braet et al., 1997 ; Zipper et al., 2001 ; Reilly et al., 2003 ; Erermis et al., 2004 ; Lobstein et al., 2004 ; Reilly and Kelly, 2011 ). Moreover, these diseases are known to track into adulthood ( Ferraro and Kelley-Moore, 2003 ; Reilly et al., 2003 ; Lifshitz, 2008 ; Reilly and Kelly, 2011 ). This progress and the assumed comorbidities emphasize the importance of studying the underlying mechanisms leading to childhood obesity. Psychological and psychophysiological research in this field emphasizes the influence of stress on eating and weight regulation.

Stress Conceptualization

Psychological stress occurs when a person appraises a situation as significant for his or her welfare and when the situation exceeds his or her available coping resources ( Lazarus and Folkman, 1986 ). This definition implicates a subjective component that can be captured only through self-report ( Krohne, 2002 ). Besides, the emotional value of the stressor needs to be taken into account. Lazarus (1993) states that stress is closely related to the conceptualization of emotions because higher levels of stress are consistently associated with higher negative affect but so far, specifically in childhood, this link is less studied in the context of emotional eating ( Cohen et al., 1995 ; Watson, 1998 ; Lemyre and Tessier, 2003 ; Mroczek and Almeida, 2004 ; Minkel et al., 2012 ; Gustafsson et al., 2013 ; Bey et al., 2018 ).

As our stress levels know day-level fluctuations within persons ( Beattie and Griffin, 2014 ), these should be studied as ‘daily hassles,’ which are situations, thoughts or events leading to negative feelings (e.g., annoyance, irritation, worry or frustration) when they occur and inform you on the difficulty or impossibility to achieve your goals and plans ( O’Connor et al., 2008 ).

Stress is associated with the development of different medical and psychological disorders ( Salim, 2014 ). Pertinent to the current study, research in children and adolescents proved that the experience of negative affect and stress is associated with weight gain, a higher waist circumference, a higher BMI and in the long term obesity ( Goodman and Whitaker, 2002 ; Koch et al., 2008 ; De Vriendt et al., 2009 ; Midei and Matthews, 2009 ; Rofey et al., 2009 ; Van Jaarsveld et al., 2009 ; Aparicio et al., 2016 ).

Stress and Eating Behavior

Psychological research shows that stress is associated with overweight and obesity through changes in weight-related health behavior, as stress activates emotional brain networks and elevates the secretion of glucocorticoids and insulin. Both the emotional brain networks and the hormones influence different aspects of our eating behavior such as our food intake, food choice and eating motives (hunger and desire eating) ( O’Connor et al., 2008 ; Dallman, 2010 ; Jastreboff et al., 2013 ).

First, regarding food intake, stress may result in under- or overeating depending on the stress source and stress intensity ( Willenbring et al., 1986 ; Steere and Cooper, 1993 ; O’Connor et al., 2008 ; Ansari and Berg-Beckhoff, 2015 ). Concerning undereating, adults and children are often found to eat less after heavy stressful events and, specifically, family stress is associated with underweight (BMI) ( Popper et al., 1989 ; Stone and Brownell, 1994 ; Oliver and Wardle, 1999 ; Stenhammar et al., 2010 ). However, evidence for overeating after experiencing stress is also found. Next to the detected underweight after family stress, Stenhammar et al. (2010) also report possible overweight after family stress, suggesting a link with respectively under- and overeating. Besides, Evers et al. (2010) and Vandewalle et al. (2017b) show in youngsters how induced negative affect leads to an increased food intake, specifically comfort foods.

Second, concerning food choice, stressor intensity is an important factor. In adults, chronic life stress is associated with the intake of more energy-dense food ( Steptoe et al., 1998 ; Torres and Nowson, 2007 ; O’Connor et al., 2008 ) a lower consumption of main meals and vegetables ( O’Connor et al., 2008 ). In a systematic review and meta-analysis on children, Hill et al. (2018) report that stress, measured via self-reports and via cortisol measures, is associated with the intake of more unhealthy and less healthy food items (8–18 years).

Third, concerning eating motives, De Vriendt et al. (2009) conclude in their review that in adolescents, stress is specifically associated with increased appetite, which can be seen as a desire for food. Therefore, a distinction should be made between ‘hunger eating motives’ (=eating out of hunger) and ‘desire to eat motives’ (=eating out of a desire to eat; eating out of a craving for food), with the latter defined as rather unhealthy eating ( Reichenberger et al., 2016 , 2018 ). Here, a recent EMA study in adults finds that time pressure is associated with more hunger eating ( Reichenberger et al., 2016 ). Goldschmidt et al. (2018) shows in the only available ecologically momentary assessment study (EMA = looking into the current real time behaviors) in obese children in their natural environment that specifically ‘desire to eat motives’ were of importance regarding overeating.

These ecological momentary assessment results ( O’Connor et al., 2008 ; Reichenberger et al., 2016 ) stress the importance of looking into the daily fluctuations in within-person stress levels to better understand the precise role of different daily stressors in eating behavior, on top of the existing measurements of stress. Besides, as eating behaviors are a daily occupation, and appear in different contexts, a diary study can further help to capture the relationship between daily hassles and different indicators of eating behavior.

However, this research in children and adolescents is lacking, but highly needed and relevant as (1) adolescence is an important developmental stage ( Giedd et al., 2009 ); (2) Goldschmidt et al. (2018) showed the importance of daily desire to eat motives in obese youngsters and (3) daily stress is positively associated with daily fluctuations in scores on emotional eating ( Vandewalle et al., 2017a ). It is hypothesized that stress- induced eating or emotional eating, defined as “eating your negative emotions away,” can be seen as a maladaptive emotion regulation (ER) strategy in children also, but this remains to be further explored ( Braet and Van Strien, 1997 ; Thayer, 2001 ; Evers et al., 2010 ).

Role of Emotion Regulation Processes

Emotion regulation strategies.

Emotion regulation refers to the actions by which persons try to influence their emotions, more specifically which emotions they experience, when and how they experience the emotions and how they show their emotions to others ( Gross, 1998 ). Emotion regulation strategies are studied most often in contexts in which persons upregulate their (stress related) negative emotions. Emotion regulation strategies influence eating behavior and health behavior ( Whiteside et al., 2007 ; Evers et al., 2010 ; Koenders and Van Strien, 2011 ), and weight gain and obesity ( De Vriendt et al., 2009 ). This association is stronger when maladaptive emotion regulation strategies are used to deal with the stressor in comparison with the use of adaptive emotion regulation strategies ( Aparicio et al., 2016 ). In a longitudinal perspective, toddlers with lower levels of emotion regulation skills (i.e., more negative emotion reactions and less adequate emotion regulation) have more overweight at the age of 10 ( Graziano et al., 2014 ), suggesting that difficulties in emotion regulation skills in 2 until 5 years old is related to the development of pediatric obesity later in life ( Graziano et al., 2010 ). Using maladaptive emotion regulation strategies is found to mediate the association between the stressor of maternal rejection and emotional eating (measured as a trait, with the DEBQ-questionnaire) in obese youngsters ( Vandewalle et al., 2014 ). On the other hand, adaptive emotion regulation strategies are related with positive health behaviors such as a higher intake of fruits and vegetables and more physical activity ( Isasi et al., 2013 ). Remarkably, whether people with high/low maladaptive emotion regulation strategies cope differently with stress is less studied.

Emotional Eating as Emotion Regulation Strategy

Emotional eating is highly prevalent in community-wide youth samples ( Michels et al., 2012 ) and in youth with obesity ( Braet and Beyers, 2009 ). Besides, emotional eating is treatment resistant, as it often persists after obesity treatment ( Vandewalle et al., 2018 ). Emotional eating can occur in the presence of a lot of different emotions ( Faith et al., 1997 ) and again, is most studied regarding stress-related emotions. In children, high scores on emotional eating have been associated with both negative feelings on physical competencies ( Braet and Van Strien, 1997 ) and experienced stress ( Nguyen-Rodriguez et al., 2009 ; Michels et al., 2012 ). Emotional eating has a short term reinforcing effect, by reducing stress-related arousal and negative affect ( Popless-Vawter et al., 1998 ; Macht, 2008 ). However, in the long-term using the strategy of ‘emotional eating’ in particular in obese persons, often instills feelings of guilt and self-anger ( Popless-Vawter et al., 1998 ), thereby intensifying the existing negative emotions ( Popless-Vawter et al., 1998 ; Gibson, 2006 ). Even more important, emotional eating will not solve the stress origins: as long as people only cope with their stress by means of emotional eating, the stressor will remain and cause psychological discomfort ( Stice et al., 2002 ; Gibson, 2006 ). This way, emotional eating can be seen as a proxy of maladaptive emotion regulation ( Michopoulos et al., 2015 ).

Emotional eating is associated with more calorie-intake ( Braet and Van Strien, 1997 ). This is of concern as emotional eating seems to evolve toward a stable trait component later in life and is of greater importance compared with other life style behaviors for explaining longitudinal weight gain ( Koenders and Van Strien, 2011 ). Persons with a high emotional eating style who are confronted with a stressor eat more high-fat food and more energy-dense meals in comparison with persons who have a low emotional eating style ( Oliver et al., 2000 ). O’Connor et al. (2008) concludes in an EMA-study that the confrontation with daily hassles is associated with more snacking, and this is stronger for individuals who have a high emotional eating style. Interesting, the presence of negative emotions decreases the desire eating in individuals with a low emotional eating style; while the negative emotions do not affect the desire eating in individuals with a high emotional eating style ( Reichenberger et al., 2016 ). This suggests that emotional eating evolves toward a stable trait that might be a moderator in the stress-eating relation.

Research Questions and Daily Diary-Design

The current study is innovative in different ways: (1) by including children and adolescents, (2) by using a daily diary design during seven consecutive days and (3) by including three indicators of eating behavior; daily hunger eating motives, desire to eat motives and snacking; and their moderating factors, emotion regulation and emotional eating. However, as using a cellphone during school time is often forbidden, the eating motives and snacking during the day will be reported after school time, which is a well-known critical phase with both school related and peer related stress ( Sotardi, 2017 ). The research question is shown in Figure 1 .

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Research question.

First, we hypothesize that emotional eating will be a proxy of maladaptive emotion regulation (based on Michopoulos et al., 2015 ). Second, we state that daily stress will be significantly associated with (1) the trajectories in desire to eat (2) the trajectories of hunger-eating and (3) the trajectories of snacking (Based on De Vriendt et al., 2009 ; Reichenberger et al., 2016 ; Vandewalle et al., 2017a ). We also hypothesize that these relations will be stronger for youth high in trait maladaptive emotion regulation and/or reporting a more pronounced emotional eating style ( O’Connor et al., 2008 ; Reichenberger et al., 2016 ).

Methodology

Participants.

Dutch- speaking children and adolescents were recruited in Belgium, Aalter and Deinze. These youngsters already participated in two previous studies: Generation 2020 study ( Van Beveren et al., 2016 , 2017 ) and the Reward- study ( De Decker et al., 2017a , b ). These are both longitudinal studies with different data collection waves over time on the emotional wellbeing of children and adolescents. During a follow-up data collection in both cohorts, participants were recruited for the current study. A total of 109 participants aged 10–17 years were recruited [ M age = 13.49; SD = 1.64; 51 boys (46.8%) and 57 girls (52.3%)]; one participant did not report their age and gender.

Study Design

Before the diary study, the participants were asked to fill out questionnaires on eating behavior and emotion regulation. Following this, the participants were required to complete the stress/eating behavior diary over seven consecutive days. EMA measures were similar to the study of Reichenberger et al. (2016) , but extended by adding the variable of snacking and the role of emotion regulation (Figure 2 ).

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Measurements.

The diary was filled out before breakfast, after school time, when the participants came home from school and before bedtime. In the current study, only the data time point after school was used, which was in most cases filled in between 5 PM and 6 PM. At 5 PM the participants received a reminder message to fill out the diary. Participants were asked to report on their snacking behavior since breakfast.

Trait Variables by Questionnaires

The Fragebogen Zur Erhebung der Emotionsregulation Bei Kindern und Jugendlichen (FEEL-KJ, Grob and Smolenski, 2005 ; A Questionnaire on Emotion Regulation Strategies in Children and Adolescents) is translated from German to Dutch and can be used in children and adolescents between 8 and 18 years old ( Braet et al., 2013 ). The questionnaire measures ER strategies for three emotions: anger, anxiety and sadness. The emotion regulation strategies for every emotion are captured by 30 items, thus the total amount of items is 90. The 15 emotion regulation strategies are divided in three categories of strategies: adaptive, maladaptive and external regulation. The adaptive emotion regulation strategies are distraction, recall a positive emotion, forget, acceptance, problem solving, re-evaluation of the situation, and cognitive reappraisal. The maladaptive strategies are giving up, withdrawing, self- devaluation, rumination and aggression. Looking for social support, controlling your emotions and expressing your emotions are the external strategies. For the Dutch and Flemish population, representative norms are available ( Braet et al., 2013 ). The psychometric qualities of the FEEL-KJ are drawn from a large sample of Dutch speaking Belgian children and adolescents between 8 and 18 years old. A good reliability and validity was found by Cracco et al. (2015) . In the current study, a good reliability was also found for both subscales adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation, respectively α = 0.957 and α = 0.831.

Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire

The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ, van Strien et al., 1986 ; Braet et al., 2008 ) assesses three eating styles: restrained eating, external eating and emotional eating. In this study only the subscale ‘emotional eating’ will be taken into account. The DEBQ contains 33 items, and is a self-report questionnaire. The items question specific eating behaviors, and should be rated on a five-point- Likert scale from 1 = never to 5 = very often. The DEBQ has been shown useful in research with children and adolescents between 7 and 17 years ( Braet et al., 2003 ). Also, good psychometric qualities are reported, such as a good reliability and external validity ( Ricciardelli and McCabe, 2001 ; Braet et al., 2008 ). The emotional eating subscale contains 13 items and has a good internal validity in overweight and normal-weight children (respectively α = 0.91 and α = 0.93) ( Braet et al., 2008 ). In the current study, a good reliability for emotional eating was found (α = 0.946).

Diary-Measures

Stress intensity.

To measure the amount of stress during a particular school day, the participants were asked to rate ‘how stressed did you feel since your breakfast?’ on a visual analog scale from 0 (not at all stressed) to 100 (extremely stressed). By asking about the period since breakfast until after school, we wanted to capture stress related to school performances and peer-related stress (social stress).

Snacking and motives of eating behavior

The after school diary questions always start with the question ‘How many snacks did you eat since breakfast?’. A snack was defined as ‘healthy and unhealthy food and/or drinks, which were not part of your main meals, e.g., lunch’. Participants had the option of choosing between ‘0 or 1,’ ‘2,’ ‘3,’ and ‘4.’ For every snack, they needed to fill out what they ate as a snack and how many, e.g., 1 apple or 1 large snicker. Using the Nobelguide, two researchers scored and discussed until agreement on the amount of Kcal, and this was double checked by a dietitian, in the case of non-accordance. For every reported snacks, the hunger eating motive and the desire to eat motive was measured using the following questions: ‘rate the extent to which you agree with the following statement of “I am hungry”’ and ‘rate the extent to which you agree with the following statement of “I desire to eat”.’ Both statements were rated on a 5-point Likert scale with 1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree. In the analysis, a calculated mean score on the amount of snacks (kcal) and a calculated mean score on hunger and desire eating motives was used.

Control variables

As control variables, age and gender were taken into account.

Reactivity Measures

It is important to know if the act of completing the daily diary impacted the behavior of the participant (reactivity, e.g., if the participants knew they had to fill out all the questions related to every snack, they might have eaten fewer snacks than normal). Therefore, at the end of the current study, reactivity was measured with a single question regarding the influence the diary had on their eating behavior. The responses were rated on a 7-point Likert scale with 1 = I ate much less, 2 = Sometimes I ate less, 3 = I ate a little bit less, 4 = filling in the diary had no effect on my eating behavior, 5 = I ate a bit more, 6 = sometimes I ate more and 7 = I ate much more.

Children and adolescents between 10 and 17 years old were recruited as part of a larger research project ( Emotion Regulation in the prevention and treatment of obesity in young adolescents).

When participants and their parents agreed on participating in the diary-study, a home visit was planned. During the first home visit the informed consents were signed by the youngsters and their parents, the trait questionnaires were explained and a smartphone was given to the participants to fill out the diary. Together with them, an example of 1 diary assessment point was filled out. Participants had to fill out the diary for 7 days, with the assessment points after breakfast, after school and before bedtime. In the current study, only the measurement point after school time is used to answer the research questions. Collecting data during this limited window of time, seven consecutive days, was based on other recent studies in youth ( Hruska et al., 2017 ; Vandewalle et al., 2017a ; Mabbe et al., 2018 ).

During the diary- data collection, the participants received at every assessment point an e-mail with the link to the diary questions as a reminder for filling in the diary. To log in on the website, every participant received a unique code, which guaranteed the anonymity of the participants. The diary questions were also given in a written format; in case the online version would give problems. When the participant did not fill in the diary three consecutive times, e-mails with the links were not sent any more to this person. At the fourth day of the diary-study, the participants received a phone call to ask how the study went, if there were difficulties and to encourage them to carry on until day 7 including an integrity check whereby the snack registration of that day was asked.

After the study, a second home visit was planned to collect the trait questionnaires and smartphones. During the second home visit, the reactivity measure was asked to the participants; and they received a film ticket as reward.

This study was approved by the Ethical Committee of Ghent University.

Data-Analysis

Data were analyzed within a multilevel framework using MLwiN ( Rasbash et al., 2009 ) because the seven consecutive days (Level 1) were nested within the individuals (Level 2). In following Nezlek (2011) recommendations, variables at Level 1 reflecting within-youth predictors were centered around the individual’s mean (group-mean centered; Enders and Tofighi, 2007 ; Nezlek, 2011 ) in order to eliminate the influence of Level 2 differences in the predictors for the analyses. Predictors at Level 2 reflecting between-youth predictors were centered around the grand mean (grand-mean centered) to improve the interpretation of the intercept values ( Nezlek, 2011 ).

As proposed by Singer and Willet (2003) , we fitted two initial models, namely the random intercept-only model and the unconditional growth model for daily desire to eat, hunger eating, and snacking prior to our main analyses. First, random-intercept-only models (Null Models) were used to estimate the variance partitioning coefficient (VPC), which reflects the proportion of variance in the dependent variable situated between youth (Level 2). In a second step, unconditional growth models ( Singer and Willet, 2003 ) were built stepwise from the Null Models by adding Time as a Level 1 predictor, reflecting whether on average there was change over time. Furthermore, we tested whether the growth rate varied between individuals by modeling the coefficient of Time random at Level 2 (i.e., random slope; unconditional growth models). The likelihood ratio test (LRT) was used to compare the goodness of fit of nested models. To evaluate the significance of the fixed and random effects the Wald test was used. Gender, age, and adjusted BMI were included as covariates of no interest in all undermentioned analyses. To test whether daily stress and trait maladaptive emotion regulation interact in explaining trajectories of desire to eat, hunger eating, and snacking respectively we extended the baseline growth models for these variables by adding daily stress, trait maladaptive emotion regulation, and their interaction to the unconditional growth models.

Throughout the week, a decreasing compliance for filling out the online diary was observed, with respectively 96, 83, and 77 youngsters filling out the diary at day 1, day 4, and day 7.

Means, standard deviations, and correlations between all variables are shown in Table 1 . Mean scores of all variables in the current sample are comparable to findings of previous studies in youth samples ( Braet and Beyers, 2009 ; Braet et al., 2013 ). When comparing the mean scores for emotional eating and emotion regulation with the gender- and age specific norms of the trait questionnaires (DEBQ and FEEL-KJ), all mean scores are not significantly different from the norm scores; except for the use of maladaptive emotion regulation strategies in boys between 13 and 17 years old [ t (32) = 3.09; p < 0.01] (Table 2 ; Braet and Beyers, 2009 ; Braet et al., 2013 ). Comparing the daily mean score for kcal-intake of snacking with previous research in girls and boys between 9 and 14 years ( Field et al., 2004 ), the mean score was significantly lower from the mean-score in their research.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Comparison of means with gender- and age specific norm scores.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Variable correlations and descriptives.

Maladaptive emotion regulation was positively correlated with emotional eating, both measured as trait variables ( r = 0.304, p < 0.01) confirming the presumption that emotional eating functions as a proxy of maladaptive emotion regulation. Additionally, all daily measurements were significantly and positively correlated, except for daily stress with daily snacking. Higher levels of daily stress were significantly associated with higher levels of daily desire to eat and hunger eating ( r = 0.267 and r = 0.274, p < 0.01, respectively) and all three indicators of eating behavior were significantly correlated (Table 1 ). Concerning the hunger eating motives and desire to eat motives, the multicollinearity was checked by the variance inflation factor (VIF). With VIF = 1.00, it was concluded that both constructs did not overlap substantially, and thus multicollinearity was not a problem.

Reactivity Measure

Concerning the reactivity measures, 98 participants filled out this question at the second home visit. Unfortunately, in 11 participants the reactivity measure was missing, due to the fact that the participants were not at home at the time of the second home visit. On the 7-point Likert scale the minimum score indicated by the participants was 2 (“Sometimes I ate less due to filling out the diary”) and the maximum score 5 (“I ate a little bit more due to filling out the diary”), respectively by 4 (3.7%) and 2 (1.8%) participants. The major group of participants stated that “filling out the diary had no influence on their snacking behavior” (Likert-score 4) ( n = 82; 75.2%), and 9.2% ( n = 10) stated that “they ate a little bit less due to filling out the diary” (Likert-score 3).

Between and Within Youth Variance of Stress and Self-Reported Eating Behavior

We first fitted random intercept-only models (i.e., Null Models) for core study variables as a baseline for more complex models. Variance estimates and standard errors (SE) are depicted in Table 3 (desire to eat), Table 4 (hunger eating), and Table 5 (snacking). For all variables, the between-youth and within-youth variance components were significant. The VPC indicated that 85.08% of the variance in daily desire to eat, 84.86% of the variance in daily hunger eating and 80.87% of the variance in daily snacking was situated between youth. Before running the main analyses, we fitted unconditional growth models for core study variables. These models revealed that levels of desire to eat, hunger eating, as well as snacking generally decreased as the week progressed.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Daily desire to eat as a function of stress, maladaptive emotion regulation, and trait emotional eating.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Daily hunger eating as a function of stress, maladaptive emotion regulation, and trait emotional eating.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Daily snacking as a function of stress, maladaptive emotion regulation, and trait emotional eating.

In addition, we fitted a random intercept-only model for the independent variable stress. Again, the between-youth [ σ u0 2 = 217.33, SE = 41.44, χ 2 (1) = 27.51, p < 0.001] and within-youth [ σ e0 2 = 328.37, SE = 21.99, χ 2 (1) = 223.06, p < 0.001] variance components were significant. The VPC indicated that 39.83% of the variance in daily stress was situated between youth. Furthermore, the unconditional growth model for stress revealed that stress-levels generally decreased as the week progressed.

Do Stress and Maladaptive Emotion Regulation Interact in Explaining Trajectories of Daily Desire to Eat?

To test our hypothesis, we extended the unconditional growth model for daily desire to eat (Model 1; Table 3 ) by adding stress and emotion regulation, as well as their interaction, to the model. Interestingly, this model (Model 2; Table 3 ) resulted in a significant improvement in model fit, χ 2 (6) = 18.49, p = 0.005. Inspection of the individual Wald test showed that daily stress significantly predict trajectories of desire to eat χ 2 (1) = 11.74, p < 0.001, revealing that with higher levels of stress, desire to eat showed a less steep decrease throughout the week whereas maladaptive emotion regulation χ 2 (1) = 0.95, p = 0.329, as well as the interaction χ 2 (1) = 0.19, p = 0.663 were not significantly predictive for these trajectories.

Do Stress and Maladaptive Emotion Regulation Interact in Explaining Trajectories of Hunger Eating?

We extended the unconditional growth model for daily hunger eating (Model 1; Table 4 ) by adding stress and emotion regulation, as well as their interaction, to the model. Interestingly, this model (Model 2; Table 4 ) resulted in a significant improvement in model fit, χ 2 (6) = 15.25, p = 0.018. Inspection of the individual Wald test showed that daily stress significantly predicted trajectories of hunger eating χ 2 (1) = 13.02, p < 0.001, revealing that with higher levels of stress, hunger eating behavior showed a less steep decrease throughout the week whereas maladaptive emotion regulation χ 2 (1) = 0.33, p = 0.563, as well as the interaction χ 2 (1) = 0.18, p = 0.669 were not significantly predictive for these trajectories.

Do Stress and Emotion Regulation Interact in Explaining Trajectories of Daily Snacking?

We extended the unconditional growth model for daily snacking (Model 1; Table 5 ) by adding stress and emotion regulation, as well as their interaction, to the model. Interestingly, this model (Model 2; Table 5 ) resulted in a significant improvement in model fit, χ 2 (6) = 15.23, p = 0.019. Inspection of the individual Wald test showed that daily stress χ 2 (1) = 0.87, p = 0.351, maladaptive emotion regulation χ 2 (1) = 1.46, p = 0.226, as well as the interaction χ 2 (1) = 0.37, p = 0.542 were not significantly predictive for these trajectories.

Do Stress and Trait Emotional Eating Interact in Explaining Trajectories of Desire to Eat?

We extended the unconditional growth model for daily desire to eat (Model 1; Table 3 ) by adding stress and emotional eating, as well as their interaction, to the model. Interestingly, this model (Model 3; Table 3 ) resulted in a significant improvement in model fit, χ 2 (6) = 20.52, p = 0.002. Inspection of the individual Wald test showed that the effect of emotional eating on trajectories of desire to eat was marginally significant χ 2 (1) = 2.97, p = 0.085, revealing that with higher levels of emotional eating, desire to eat showed a less steep decrease throughout the week. The interaction between daily stress and emotional eating was not significant χ 2 (1) = 0.33, p = 0.566.

Do Stress and Trait Emotional Eating Interact in Explaining Trajectories of Hunger Eating?

We extended the unconditional growth model for daily hunger eating (Model 1; Table 4 ) by adding stress and emotional eating, as well as their interaction, to the model. Interestingly, this model (Model 3; Table 4 ) resulted in a significant improvement in model fit, χ 2 (6) = 14.94, p = 0.021. Inspection of the individual Wald test showed that despite the significant effect of daily stress, emotional eating χ 2 (1) = 0.14, p = 0.707, as well as the interaction between stress and emotional eating, χ 2 (1) = 0.03, p = 0.862, was not significantly predictive for these trajectories.

Do Stress and Trait Emotional Eating Interact in Explaining Trajectories of Daily Snacking?

We extended the unconditional growth model for daily snacking (Model 1; Table 5 ) by adding stress and EE, as well as their interaction, to the model. Interestingly, this model (Model 3; Table 5 ) resulted in a significant improvement in model fit, χ 2 (6) = 139.69, p < 0.001. Inspection of the individual Wald test showed that despite the significant effect of daily stress, emotional eating χ 2 (1) = 0.72, p = 0.396, was not significantly predictive for these trajectories. The interaction term between stress and emotional eating was only marginally significant χ 2 (1) = 2.66, p = 0.102.

The current study aimed to investigate the relationship between daily stress and eating behavior in children and adolescents (10–17 years old) using a daily diary-design. Combining a daily diary-design with the use of self-report trait questionnaires allowed us to look into the moderating role of emotion regulation and emotional eating.

First, we found that maladaptive emotion regulation was positively correlated with emotional eating, meaning emotional eating can be seen as a proxy of maladaptive emotion regulation, which is in line with Evers et al. (2010) and Michopoulos et al. (2015) . Besides, daily stress was positively correlated with daily desire to eat and hunger eating; but not with daily snacking; and these three indicators of daily eating behavior were all significantly positively correlated. The positive correlation between daily stress on the one hand and desire to eat motives and hunger eating motives on the other hand was expected, as (1) the item on ‘desire to eat motives’ is seen as a desire or craving for food after experiencing stress and thus may be a way of regulating the experienced stress and (2) the item on ‘hunger to eat motives’ is seen as eating out of hunger after experiencing stress; and previous research showed that stress, operationalized as a hyperactive cortisol axis, contributes to higher levels of hunger feelings ( Gluck et al., 2004 ; Goldschmidt et al., 2018 ). The correlations are in line with the reported positive associations between levels of stress and levels of desire to eat- and hunger eating motives by De Vriendt et al. (2009) in children and Reichenberger et al. (2016) in adults. In contrary to previous findings ( O’Connor et al., 2008 ; Reichenberger et al., 2016 ), we could not find an association between daily stress and snacking; maybe due to specific characteristics of the study population: children instead of adults. Children and youngsters might not have the autonomy to decide what they will eat, especially snacks. Gevers et al. (2015) found that most parents both communicate about food and at the same time restrict their children on snacking behavior.

All three indicators of eating behavior showed bilateral significant positive correlations, which is in line with our expectations. Although the motivation differs in desire and hunger eating, a positive correlation is not unexpected since in both hedonic and homeostatic eating the ghrelin secretion (hunger hormone) is elevated, and the cholecystokinin-33 secretion (satiety hormone) is decreased ( Monteleone et al., 2013 ). Concluding, variables measured on trait questionnaires (FEEL-KJ and DEBQ) correlated bilateral; and the variables measured on a daily basis (daily stress and the indicators of eating behavior) correlated with each other, but no correlations between the trait variables and daily measurements were found. For this, a potential explanation could be found in that the used questionnaires were not sensitive enough for capturing these momentary daily fluctuations.

Second, we found evidence for the hypothesis that daily stress was significantly associated with the trajectories of desire to eat and hunger eating motives, which is in line with De Vriendt et al. (2009) and Reichenberger et al. (2016) . With higher levels of daily stress, desire to eat and hunger eating motives showed a less steep decrease throughout the week. As the trajectories of the eating behavior indicators decreased significantly throughout the week for every participant, the steepness of these decreases were of interest. The decreases of the hunger eating motives trajectories and of the desire to eat motives trajectories were significantly less steep in persons reporting higher levels of stress than in persons reporting lower levels of stress. This effect was not found for snacking.

Third, for both eating motives and snacking, no effect of maladaptive emotion regulation nor an interaction between daily stress and maladaptive emotion regulation was found. These findings are in contradiction with Evers et al. (2010) , Vandewalle et al. (2014) , and Aparicio et al. (2016) . A possible explanation is the methodology of the study, as above mentioned studies are longitudinal, cross-sectional (questionnaires) or experimental studies while the current research is a diary study. Still, the moderating factor, maladaptive emotion regulation, was measured as a trait variable and therefore might not be sensitive enough to capture momentary daily effects. Next, the participants only filled out the diary during 7 days, three times a day, but only the measurement point after school was taking into account in the analysis. We could question if using one data time point a day during 7 days is enough to capture the momentary daily fluctuations. To approach these methodological shortcomings, it is recommended to include a daily measurement of emotion regulation and to include signal- or event-contingent sampling.

Fourth, we found marginally significant evidence for the hypothesis that daily stress in interaction with trait emotional eating is associated with the trajectories of desire to eat and snacking; but not for hunger eating. These results mean that in youngsters with a high emotional eating style, when experiencing high stress, a less steep decrease in desire to eat and snacking occurs in comparison with youngsters with a lower emotional eating style. These results are in line with O’Connor et al. (2008) and Reichenberger et al. (2016) .

Fifth, analyses of the daily diary data also revealed that daily stress and daily eating behavior decreased as the week progressed. This was not expected and has to be explored in the future. One of the explanations can be the possibility of the contributing factor of the stress-levels fitting the day of the week. These stress-levels may play a role in the findings in this study ( Areni et al., 2011 ; van Roekel et al., 2015 ). In future research, it is recommended to start the diary study on different days, to control for this order effect.

This study has several notable strengths. First, we used a daily diary design, enabling a more momentary inspection of the relation between stress and eating behavior in a naturalistic environment. Having a good reliability, validity and generalizability, diary studies are able to determine experiences, mood, behavior and contextual factors more detailed ( Moskowitz and Young, 2006 ; Suveg et al., 2010 ; Engel et al., 2016 ; Burke et al., 2017 ; Goldschmidt et al., 2018 ). Second, we recruited a fairly large sample of youngsters between 10 and 17 years of age given that research in this age group is lacking. Yet, researching the effects of stress on wellbeing in this critical developmental period is of utmost importance given the heightened risk for developing overweight and eating pathology in particular ( Giedd et al., 2009 ). Third, because of the highly demanding design of the study for the youngsters (diary for 7 days), a lot of efforts by the researchers were made to increase the persevered motivation of the youngsters. During a home visit the rationale was explained, a reward at the end of the study was promised and the youngsters received a personalized smartphone as incentive during the study. During the study reminders were sent for all assessing points and youngsters received a motivating phone call in between.

Despite these strengths, several limitations of the current study warrant discussion. The first limitation of the present study design is the limited number of data points each day, more specifically at breakfast, after school and before bedtime. Using signal- or event-contingent data sampling would be better to capture the daily relationship between stress and eating behavior. However, a diary-study for 7 consecutive days might be a long period for children and adolescents between 10 and 17 years old and a great burden. This was confirmed by the missing values in the diary reports. As the ability and willingness of participants are determining factors for the success of a diary study, only three diary data points were used ( Moskowitz and Young, 2006 ; Engel et al., 2016 ). Besides, practical barriers prevented us from using signal- or event contingent sampling as schools did not give an authorization to fill out the diary during school time. To answer the research questions, only one measurement point a day was included in the analysis, more specifically the data point after school. First of all, two important stressors for youngsters, school and peer related stress ( Sotardi, 2017 ) could be captured at that data point. Second, previous research showed the importance of the food availability preceding the snacking, which is higher at home (or the way to home) in contrast with a school environment. Due to the age of the participants, 10–17 years, and the possibility of having a greater food availability at home (or the way to home), the choice was made to include only this data time point here ( Chopra et al., 2002 ; Johnson et al., 2011 ; Pearson et al., 2014 ; De Decker et al., 2017a , b ). Whether emotional eating might be even more prevalent later in the evening, remains to be studied.

A second limitation of the study concerns the use of self-report measurements of eating behavior. In future research, it is recommended to use more objective methods, such as taking pictures of the snacks participants ate. This requires the use of event-contingent data sampling.

A third limitation, in addition to the one-time point, is the retrospective character of these daily measures. As mentioned above, including a signal-contingent design of the daily measures would be more suitable in order to investigate the causal daily relationship between stress and eating behavior. A fourth limitation concerns the little variance in the emotion regulation and emotional eating scores. All mean scores are in accordance with the norm scores of the questionnaires, except for maladaptive emotion regulation in boys between 12 and 18 years ( Braet and Beyers, 2009 ; Braet et al., 2013 ). In future research, it might be interesting to include youngsters with overweight or obesity, as emotional eating is more prevalent among overweight and obese youngsters ( Braet and Beyers, 2009 ). As mentioned above, a fifth limitation concerns the same start day for all participants of the diary- measures, namely on Monday. To control for week-effects, it might be better to randomize the start day for filling in the diary over the participants ( Areni et al., 2011 ; van Roekel et al., 2015 ). Sixth, in this study, only intensity of stress was included as independent variable, while also the source of stress is of importance ( O’Connor et al., 2008 ). In future research, it is recommended to include sources of daily hassles to have more specified information. Different types of stress are of interest in youngsters, e.g., Family problems; Problems with Friends; Parental Rejection; School Related Stress; Time Pressure; Concerns about Body; Romantic concerns; Sports Pressure and General self-worth ( Treffers et al., 2002 ; O’Connor et al., 2008 ; Reichenberger et al., 2016 ). Seventh, in the current study, stress intensity was measured subjectively. As the variance within persons is rather small, the question could raise if youngsters have difficulties reporting on their stress level. In the future, we will analyze with more stringent research whether the level of the stress experience of children is related with more objective measurements, like heart rate variability. Eighth, all daily variables (e.g., stress intensity, hunger- and desire to eat motives and snacking) were measured with only one item. Although, this phenomenon is seen in different daily diary studies ( O’Connor et al., 2008 ; Reichenberger et al., 2018 ), future research should use more questions to operationalize the variables.

To conclude, we found that daily stress is significantly associated with the trajectories of desire to eat and hunger eating motives. But no moderation effects of maladaptive emotion regulation were found; while emotional eating has marginally significant effects on desire to eat and snacking. In the research on stress and eating behavior, there are still a lot of gaps. The current research is a first step in reducing these gaps by investigating the underlying mechanism in the relationship between daily stress and eating behavior as a predictor of weight gain by including the moderators of emotion regulation, and more specifically emotional eating.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of Ghent University with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Ethical Committee of Ghent University.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

This work was supported by a BOF Grant (BOF.24J.2016.0007.02).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Ansari, W. E., and Berg-Beckhoff, G. (2015). Nutritional correlates of perceived stress among university students in Egypt. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 12, 14164–14176. doi: 10.3390/ijerph121114164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aparicio, E., Canals, J., Arija, V., De Henauw, S., and Michels, N. (2016). The role of emotion regulation in childhood obesity: implications for prevention and treatment. Nutr. Res. Rev. 29, 17–29. doi: 10.1017/S0954422415000153

Areni, C. S., Burger, M., and Zlatevska, N. (2011). Factors affecting the extent of Monday blues: evidence from a meta-analysis. Psychol. Rep. 109, 723–733. doi: 10.2466/13.20.PR0.109.6.723-733

Beattie, L., and Griffin, B. (2014). Day-level fluctuations in stress and engagement in response to workplace incivility: a diary study. J. Work Stress 28, 124–142. doi: 10.1080/02678373.2014.898712

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bey, G. S., Waring, M. E., Jesdale, B. M., and Person, S. D. (2018). Gendered race modification of the association between chronic stress and depression among black and white US. Adults. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 88, 151–160. doi: 10.1037/ort0000301

Braet, C., and Beyers, W. (2009). Subtyping children and adolescents who are overweight: different symptomatology and treatment outcomes. J. Consul. Clin. Psychol. 77, 814–824. doi: 10.1037/a0016304

Braet, C., Claus, L., Goossens, L., Moens, E., Van Vlierberghe, L., and Soetens, B. (2008). Differences in eating style between overweight and normal weight youngsters. J. Health Psychol. 13, 733–743. doi: 10.1177/1359105308093850

Braet, C., Cracco, E., Theuwis, L., Grob, A., and Smolenski, C. (2013). Feel-KJ Vragenlijst Over Emotieregulatie bij Kinderen en Jongeren, Handleiding. Available at: Vub.ac.be/PE/Documenten/ATOM/FEEL.pdf

Google Scholar

Braet, C., Mervielde, I., and Vandereycken, W. (1997). Psychological aspects of childhood obesity: a controlled study in a clinical and nonclinical sample. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 22, 59–71. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/22.1.59

Braet, C., Tanghe, A., De Bode, P., Franckx, H., and Van Winckel, M. (2003). Inpatient treatment of obese children: a multicomponent programme without stringent calorie restriction. Eur. J. Pediatr. 162, 391–396. doi: 10.1007/s00431-003-1155-5

Braet, C., and Van Strien, T. (1997). Assessment of emotional, externally induced and restrained eating behaviour in nine to twelve-year-old obese and non-obese children. Behav. Res. Ther. 35, 863–873. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00045-4

Burke, L. E., Shiffman, S., Music, E., Styn, M., Kriska, A., Smailagic, A., et al. (2017). Ecological momentary assessment in behavioral research: addressing technological and human participant challenges. J. Med. Internet Res. 19:e77. doi: 10.2196/jmir.7138

Chopra, M., Galbraith, S., and Darnton-Hill, I. (2002). A global response to a global problem: the epidemic of overnutrition. Bull. World Health Organ. 80, 952–958. doi: 10.1590/S0042-96862002001200009

Cohen, S., Kessler, C., and Gordon, L. U. (1995). “Strategies for measuring stress in studies of psychiatric and physical disorders,” in Measuring Stress: A Guide for Health and Social Scientists , eds S. Cohen, R. C. Kessler, and L. G. Underwood (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 3–28.

Cracco, E., Van Durme, K., and Braet, C. (2015). Validation of the FEEL-KJ: an instrument to measure emotion regulation strategies in children and adolescents. PLoS One 10:e0137080. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137080

Dallman, M. F. (2010). Stress-induced obesity and the emotional nervous system. Trends Endocrinol. Metab. 21, 159–165. doi: 10.1016/j.tem.2009.10.004

De Decker, A., Verbeken, S., Sioen, I., Moens, E., Braet, C., and De Henauw, S. (2017a). Fat tissue accretion in children and adolescents: interplay between food responsiveness, gender, and the home availability of snacks. Front. Psychol. 7:2041. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02041

De Decker, A., Verbeken, S., Sioen, I., Van Lippelvelde, W., Braet, C., Eiben, G., et al. (2017b). Palatable food consumption in children: interplay between (food) reward motivation and the home food environment. Eur. J. Pediatr. 176, 465–474. doi: 10.1007/s00431-017-2857-4

De Vriendt, T., Moreno, L. A., and De Henauw, S. (2009). Chronic stress and obesity in adolescents: scientific evidence and methodological issues for epidemiological research. Nutr. Metab. Cardiovasc. Dis. 19, 511–519. doi: 10.1016/j.numecd.2009.02.009

Enders, C. K., and Tofighi, D. (2007). Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: a new look at an old issue. Psychol. Methods 12, 121–138. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.2.121

Engel, S. G., Crosby, R. D., Thomas, G., Bond, D., Lavender, J. M., Mason, T., et al. (2016). Ecological momentary assessment in eating disorder and obesity research: a review of the recent literature. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 18:37. doi: 10.1007/s11920-016-0672-7

Erermis, S., Cetin, N., Tamar, M., Bukusoglu, N., Akdeniz, F., and Goksen, D. (2004). Is obesity a risk factor for psychopathology among adolescents? J. Pediatr. Int. 46, 296–301. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-200x.2004.01882.x

Evers, C., Stok, F. M., and De Ridder, T. D. (2010). Feeding your feelings: emotion regulation strategies and emotional eating. Pers. Soc. Pychol. Bull. 36, 792–804. doi: 10.1177/0146167210371383

Faith, M. S., Allison, D. B., and Geliebter, A. (1997). “Emotional eating and obesity: theoretical considerations and practical recommendations,” in Overweight and Weight Management: The Health Professional’s Guide to Understanding and Practice , ed. S. Dalton (Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen Publishers), 439–465.

Ferraro, K. F., and Kelley-Moore, J. A. (2003). Cumulative disadvantage and health: long-term consequences of obesity? J. Am. Sociol. Rev. 68, 707–729. doi: 10.2307/1519759

Field, A. E., Austin, S. B., Gillman, M. W., Rosner, B., Rockett, H. R., and Colditz, G. A. (2004). Snack food intake does not predict weight change among children and adolescents. Int. J. Obes. 28, 1210–1216. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802762

Gevers, D. W. M., Kremers, S. P. J., De Vries, N. K., and Van Assema, P. (2015). Patterns of food parenting practices and children’s intake of energy-dense snack foods. Nutrients 7, 4093–4106. doi: 10.3390/nu7064093

Gibson, E. L. (2006). Emotional influences on food choice: sensory, physiological and psychological pathways. J. Physiol. Behav. 89, 53–61. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.01.024

Giedd, J. N., Keshavan, M., and Paus, T. (2009). Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 947–957. doi: 10.1038/nrn2513

Gluck, M. D., Geliebter, A., Hung, J., and Yahav, E. (2004). Cortisol, hunger, and desire to binge eat following a cold stress test in obese women with binge eating disorder. J. Psychosom. Med. 66, 876–881. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000143637.63508.47

Goldschmidt, A. B., Smith, K. E., Crosby, R. D., Boyd, H. K., Dougherty, E., Engel, S. G., et al. (2018). Ecological momentary assessment of maladaptive eating in children and adolescents with overweight or obesity. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 51, 549–557. doi: 10.1002/eat.22864

Goodman, E., and Whitaker, R. C. (2002). A prospective study of the role of depression in the development and persistence of adolescent obesity. J. Pediatr. 110, 497–504. doi: 10.1542/peds.110.3.497

Graziano, P. A., Calkins, S. D., and Keane, S. P. (2010). Toddler self-regulation skills predict risk for pediatric obesity. Int. J. Obes. 34, 633–641. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2009.288

Graziano, P. A., Kelleher, R., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P., and Brien, M. O. (2014). Predicting weight outcomes in preadolescence: the role of toddlers’ self-regulation skills and the temperament dimension of pleasure. Int. J. Obes. 37, 937–942. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2012.165

Grob, A., and Smolenski, C. (2005). FEEL-KJ: Manual und Fragebogen zur Erhebung der Emotionsregulation bei Kindern und Jugendlichen. Bern. Available at: http://edoc.unibas.ch/dok/A6223168

Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2, 271–299. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271

Gustafsson, H., Skoog, T., Podlog, L., Lundqvist, C., and Wagnsson, S. (2013). Hope and athlete burnout: stress and affect as mediators. J. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 14, 640–649. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2013.03.008

Hill, D. C., Moss, R. H., Sykes-Muskett, B., Conner, M., and O’Connor, D. B. (2018). Stress and eating behaviors in children and adolescents: systematic review and meta-analysis. Appetite 123, 14–22. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.11.109

Hruska, L. C., Zelic, K. J., Dickson, K. S., and Ciesla, J. A. (2017). Adolescents’ Co-rumination and stress predict affective changes in a daily-diary paradigm. Int. J. Psychol. 52, 372–380. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12227

Isasi, C. R., Ostrovsky, N. W., and Wills, T. (2013). The association of emotion regulation with lifestyle behaviors in inner-city adolescents. J. Eat. Behav. 14, 518–521. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.07.009

Jastreboff, A. M., Sinha, R., Lacadie, C., Small, D. M., Sherwin, R. S., and Potenza, M. N. (2013). Neural correlates of stress-and food cue-induced food craving in obesity. J. Diabetes Care 36, 394–402. doi: 10.2337/dc12-1112

Johnson, L., van Jaarsveld, C. H., and Wardle, J. (2011). Individual and family environment correlates differ for consumption of core and non-core foods in children. Br. J. Nutr. 105, 950–959. doi: 10.1017/S0007114510004484

Koch, F., Sepa, A., and Ludvigsson, J. (2008). Psychological stress and obesity. J. Pediatr. 153, 839–844. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.06.016

Koenders, P. G., and Van Strien, T. (2011). Emotional eating, rather than lifestyle behavior, drives weight gain in a prospective study in 1562 employees. J. Occup. Environ. Med. 53, 1287–1293. doi: 10.1097/JOM.0b013e31823078a2

Krohne, H. W. (2002). Stress and Coping Theories. Available at: http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/schuez/folien/Krohne_Stress.pdf

Lazarus, R. S. (1993). From psychological stress to the emotions: a history of changing outlooks. J. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 44, 1–22. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.44.020193.000245

Lazarus, R. S., and Folkman, S. (1986). “Cognitive theories of stress and the issue of circularity,” in Dynamics of Stress. Physiological, Psychological, and Social Perspectives , eds M. H. Appley and R. Trumbull (New York, NY: Plenum), 63–80.

Lemyre, L., and Tessier, R. (2003). Measuring psychological stress. Can. Fam. Physician 48, 1159–1160.

Lifshitz, F. (2008). Obesity in children. J. Clin. Res. Pediatr. Endocrinol. 1, 53–60. doi: 10.4008/jcrpe.v1i2.35

Lobstein, T., Baur, L., and Uauy, R. (2004). Obesity in children and young people: a crisis in public health. J. Obes. Rev. 5, 4–85. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2004.00133.x

Mabbe, E., Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., van der Kaap-Deeder, J., and Mouratidis, A. (2018). Day-to-day variation in autonomy-supportive and psychologically controlling parenting: the role of parents’ daily experiences of need satisfaction and need frustration. Parenting 18, 86–109. doi: 10.1080/15295192.2018.1444131

Macht, M. (2008). How emotions affect eating: a five- way model. Appetite 50, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.07.002

Michels, N., Sioen, I., Braet, C., Eiben, G., Hebestreit, A., Huybrechts, I., et al. (2012). Stress, emotional eating behaviour and dietary patterns in children. Appetite 59, 762–769. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.08.010

Michopoulos, V., Powers, A., Moore, C., Villarreal, S., Ressler, K. J., and Bradley, B. (2015). The mediating role of emotion dysregulation and depression on the relationship between childhood trauma exposure and emotional eating. Appetite 91, 129–136. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.03.036

Midei, A. J., and Matthews, K. A. (2009). Social relationships and negative emotional traits are associated with central adiposity and arterial stiffness in healthy adolescents. J. Health Psychol. 28, 347–353. doi: 10.1037/a0014214

Minkel, J. D., Banks, S., Htaik, O., Moreta, M. C., Jones, C. W., McGlinchey, E. L., et al. (2012). Sleep deprivation and stressors: evidence for elevated negative affect in response to mild stressors when sleep deprived. J. Emot. 12, 1015–1020. doi: 10.1037/a0026871

Monteleone, P., Piscitelli, F., Scognamigli, P., Monteleone, A. M., Perillo, D., Cantressli, B., et al. (2013). Gastroenteric hormone responses to hedonic eating in healthy humans. J. Psychoneuroendocrinol. 38, 1435–1441. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.12.009

Moskowitz, D. S., and Young, S. N. (2006). Ecological momentary assessment: what it is and why it is a method of the future in clinical psychopharmacology. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 31, 13–20.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mroczek, D. K., and Almeida, D. M. (2004). The effect of daily stress, personality and age on daily negative affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 72, 355–378. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00265.x

Nezlek, J. B. (2011). Multilevel Modeling for Social and Personality Psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications Ltd. doi: 10.4135/9781446287996

Nguyen-Rodriguez, S. T., Unger, J. B., and Sprijtz-Metz, D. (2009). Psychological determinants of emotional eating in adolescence. J. Eat. Disord. 17, 211–224. doi: 10.1080/10640260902848543

O’Connor, D. B., Jones, F., Connor, M., McMillan, B., and Ferguson, E. (2008). Effects of daily hassles and eating style on eating behavior. J. Health Psychol. 27, 20–31.

Oliver, G., and Wardle, J. (1999). Perceived effects of stress on food choice. J. Physiol. Behav. 66, 511–515. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00322-9

Oliver, G., Wardle, J., and Gibson, E. L. (2000). Stress and food choice: a laboratory study. J. Psychosom. Med. 62, 853–865. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200011000-00016

Pearson, N., Biddle, S. J., Williams, L., Worsley, A., Crawford, D., and Ball, K. (2014). Adolescent television viewing and unhealthy snack food consumption: the mediating role of home availability of unhealthy snack foods. J. Public Health Nutr. 17, 317–323. doi: 10.1017/S1368980012005204

Popless-Vawter, S., Brandau, C., and Straub, J. (1998). Triggers of overeating and related intervention strategies for women who weight cycle. Appl. Nurs. Res. 11, 69–76. doi: 10.1016/S0897-1897(98)80207-3

Popper, R., Smits, G., Meiselman, H. L., and Hirsch, E. (1989). Eating in combat: a survey of U.S. marines. J. Mil. Med. 154, 619–623. doi: 10.1093/milmed/154.12.619

Rasbash, J., Charlton, C., Browne, W. J., Healy, M., and Cameron, B. (2009). MlwiN Version 2.1. Centre for Multilevel Modelling. Bristol: University of Bristol, 1.

Reichenberger, J., Kuppens, P., Liedlgruber, M., Wilhelm, F. H., Tiefengrabner, M., Ginzinger, S., et al. (2016). No haste, more taste: an EMA-study on the effects of stress, negative and positive emotions on eating behavior. Biol. Psychol. 131, 54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.09.002

Reichenberger, J., Richard, A., Smyth, J. M., Fischer, D., Pollatos, O., and Blechert, J. (2018). It’s craving time: time of day effects on momentary hunger and food craving in daily life. Nutrition 55–56, 15–20. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2018.03.048

Reilly, J., and Kelly, J. (2011). Long-term impact of overweight and obesity in childhood and adolescence on morbidity and premature mortality in adulthood: systematic review. Int. J. Obes. 35, 891–898. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2010.222

Reilly, J., Methven, E., McDowell, Z., Hacking, B., Alexander, D., Stewart, L., et al. (2003). Health consequences of obesity. Arch. Dis. Child. 88, 748–752. doi: 10.1136/adc.88.9.748

Ricciardelli, L. A., and McCabe, M. P. (2001). Children’s body image concerns and eating disturbance: a review of the literature. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 21, 325–344. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(99)00051-3

Rofey, D., Kolko, R., Iosif, A., Silk, J. S., Bost, J. E., Feng, W., et al. (2009). A longitudinal study of childhood depression and anxiety in relation to weight gain. J. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 40, 517–526. doi: 10.1007/s10578-009-0141-1

Salim, S. (2014). Oxidative stress and psychological disorders. J. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 12, 140–147. doi: 10.2174/1570159X11666131120230309

Singer, J. D., and Willet, J. B. (eds). (2003). “A framework for investigating change over time,” in Applied Longitudinal Data analysis: Modeling Change and Event Occurencence , (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 3–15. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195152968.003.0001

Sotardi, V. A. (2017). Exploring school stress in middle childhood: interpretations, experiences, and coping. J. Pastor. Care Educ. 35, 13–27. doi: 10.1080/02643944.2016.1269360

Steere, J., and Cooper, P. J. (1993). The effects on eating of dietary restraints, anxiety and hunger. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 13, 211–219. doi: 10.1016/j.jnutbio.2011.01.005

Stenhammar, C., Olsson, G. M., Bahmanyar, S., Hulting, A.-L., Wettergren, B., Edlund, B., et al. (2010). Family stress and BMI in young children. J. Acta Paediatr. 99, 1205–1212. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2010.01776.x

Steptoe, A., Lipsey, Z., and Wardle, J. (1998). Stress, hassles and variation in alcohol consumption, food choice and physical exercise: a diary study. Br. J. Health Psychol. 3, 51–63. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8287.1998.tb00555.x

Stice, E., Presnell, K., and Spangler, D. (2002). Risk factors for binge eating onset in adolescent girls: a 2-year prospective investigation. J. Health Psychol. 21, 131–138. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.21.2.131

Stone, A. A., and Brownell, K. (1994). The stress-eating paradox: multiple daily measurements in adult males and females. J. Psychol. Health 9, 425–436. doi: 10.1080/08870449408407469

Suveg, C., Payne, M., Thomassin, K., and Jacob, M. L. (2010). Electronic diaries: a feasible method of assessing emotional experiences in youth? J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 32, 57–67. doi: 10.1007/s10862-009-9162-0

Thayer, R. (2001). How People Regulate Mood with Food and Exercise. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Torres, S. J., and Nowson, C. A. (2007). Relationship between stress, eating behavior, and obesity. J. Nutr. 23, 887–894. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2007.08.008

Treffers, P. D. A., Goedhart, A. W., Veerman, J. W., Van den Bergh, B. R. H., Anckaert, L., and De Rycke, L. (2002). Competentiebelevingsschaal voor adolescenten. Tijdschr. Psychiatr. 46, 468–469.

Van Beveren, M. L., Harding, K., and Braet, C. (2017). Don’t worry, be happy: the role of positive emotionality and adaptive emotion regulation strategies for youth depressive symptoms. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 57, 18–41. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12151

Van Beveren, M. L., Mezulis, A., Wante, L., and Braet, C. (2016). Joint contributions of negative emotionality, positive emotionality, and effortful control on depressive symptoms in youth. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2016.1233499 [Epub ahead of print].

Van Jaarsveld, C. H. M., Fidler, J. A., Steptoe, A., Boniface, D., and Wardle, J. (2009). Perceived stress and weight gain in adolescence: a longitudinal analysis. J. Obes. 17, 2155–2161. doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.183

van Roekel, E., Ha, T., Verhagen, M., Kuntsche, E., Scholte, R. H., and Engels, R. C. (2015). Social stress in early adolescents’ daily lives: associations with affect and loneliness. J. Adolesc. 45, 274–283. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2015.10.012

van Strien, T., Frijters, J. E. R., Bergers, G. P. A., and Defares, P. B. (1986). The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating behavior. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 5, 295–315. doi: 10.1002/1098-108X(198602)5:2<295::AID-EAT2260050209>3.0.CO;2-T

Vandewalle, J., Mabbe, E., Debeuf, T., Braet, C., and Moens, E. (2017a). The daily relation between parental rejection and emotional eating in youngsters: a diary study. Front. Psychol. 8:691. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00691

Vandewalle, J., Moens, E., Bosmans, G., and Braet, C. (2017b). The effect of parental rejection on the emotional eating behaviour of youngsters: a laboratory-based study. Appetite 108, 219–225. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.10.007

Vandewalle, J., Moens, E., and Braet, C. (2014). Comprehending emotional eating in obese youngsters: the role of parental rejection and emotion regulation. Int. J. Obes. 38, 525–530. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.233

Vandewalle, J., Moens, E., Debeuf, T., and Braet, C. (2018). Parental rejection as a predictor of emotional eating after inpatient weight loss treatment for youngsters. J. Behav. Change 35, 217–227.

Watson, D. (1998). Intraindividual and interindividual analyses of positive and negative affect: their relation to health complaints, perceived stress and daily activities. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1020–1030. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1020

Whiteside, U., Chen, E., Neighbors, C., Hunter, D., Lo, T., and Larimer, M. (2007). Difficulties regulation emotions: do binge eaters have fewer strategies to modulate and tolerate negative affect? J. Eat. Behav. 8, 162–169. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2006.04.001

Willenbring, M. L., Levine, A. S., and Morley, J. E. (1986). Stress induced eating and food preference in humans: a pilot study. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 5, 855–864. doi: 10.1002/1098-108X(198607)5:5<855::AID-EAT2260050507>3.0.CO;2-O

World Health Organization [WHO] (2018). Obesity and Overweight. Available at: http://www.who.int/en/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/obesity-and-overweight

Zipper, E., Vila, G., Dabbas, M., Bertrand, C., Mouren-Siméoni, M., Robert, J., et al. (2001). Obesity in children and adolescents, mental disorders and familial psychopathology. Presse Méd. 30, 1489–1495.

Keywords : daily diary, stress, eating behavior, emotion regulation, emotional eating

Citation: Debeuf T, Verbeken S, Van Beveren M-L, Michels N and Braet C (2018) Stress and Eating Behavior: A Daily Diary Study in Youngsters. Front. Psychol. 9:2657. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02657

Received: 13 July 2018; Accepted: 10 December 2018; Published: 21 December 2018.

Reviewed by:

Copyright © 2018 Debeuf, Verbeken, Van Beveren, Michels and Braet. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Taaike Debeuf, [email protected]

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Examining the effects of stress on eating behaviour in children and young adults.

--> Moss, Rachael Hannah (2019) Examining the effects of stress on eating behaviour in children and young adults. PhD thesis, University of Leeds.

In adults, stress has been found to initiate both increases and decreases in eating behaviours, and has been found to affect the types of foods consumed. However, there is a paucity of research on stress and eating amongst children and few studies have investigated the role of other factors such as positive and negative emotions. This thesis explored the impact that stress and emotions had on the eating behaviours of children aged 8-12 years in comparison to that of undergraduate students aged 18-49 years. To explore this, four studies were conducted, of which two utilised a repeated measures daily diary design where stress was also measured using salivary cortisol. Stress was measured by assessing daily hassles and emotions were measured by assessing positive and negative emotions together with daily uplifts. To explore eating behaviour, self-reported between-meal snacking was the main outcome variable. Overall, stress was found to be associated with the snacking behaviours of both children and undergraduate students (Studies 1 and 2). Children provided more snack responses for positive emotions, whereas undergraduate students responded more for negative emotions (Study 1). Total hassles were found to be positively related to total snack consumption. The impact of total hassles on total snack consumption was moderated by cortisol reactivity within children (Study 3). Amongst undergraduate students, the relationship between total hassles and unhealthy (high in sugar and fat) snack consumption was moderated by cortisol awakening response (Study 4). Future research should consider concerns of under/over-reporting snack behaviours and the difficulty of ensuring newly formed stress measures are valid. There are however additional individual factors (e.g., gender and ethnicity) that could influence the stress-eating relationship. It would be useful if researchers explored developing individual coping strategies to reduce the effects of stress and emotion on eating behaviours.

--> Final eThesis - complete (pdf) -->

Filename: Moss_RH_Psychology_PhD_2019.pdf

Creative Commons Licence

Embargo Date:

[img]

You do not need to contact us to get a copy of this thesis. Please use the 'Download' link(s) above to get a copy. You can contact us about this thesis . If you need to make a general enquiry, please see the Contact us page.

-

  • < Previous

Home > GRAD > DISSERTATION > 235

Dissertations

The relation of anxiety, depression, and stress to binge eating behavior.

Diane L. Rosenbaum , University of Missouri-St. Louis

Document Type

Dissertation

Doctor of Philosophy

Psychology, Clinical-Community

Date of Defense

Graduate advisor.

Kamila S. White, PhD

Steven Bruce, PhD

Zoe Peterson, PhD

Susan Kashubeck-West, PhD

Binge eating is an impairing and prevalent problem. Theories of binge eating suggest negative affect is of primary importance in the development and maintenance of binge eating behavior. To date, investigations of binge eating have often examined depression, however relatively little is known about other psychological factors. The current study aimed to extend the literature by examining several psychological factors in relation to binge eating behavior. Specifically, the relation between binge eating behavior and three psychological factors, depression, anxiety and stress, were examined. Data were collected via online surveys from a community sample of men and women of diverse backgrounds. The main study hypotheses were supported, indicating a unique relation between anxiety and binge eating, and between stress and binge eating, independent of the impact of depression. Additionally, secondary analyses supported the role of cognitive avoidance as a mediator in the relation of anxiety and binge eating. Hostility was also examined in relation to binge eating. The results of this study suggest a need for greater inclusivity (i.e., anxiety, stress) of dimensions of negative affect in examinations of binge eating. Further, focused attention regarding the role of anxiety may be important for future binge eating research.

OCLC Number

Recommended citation.

Rosenbaum, Diane L., "The Relation of Anxiety, Depression, and Stress to Binge Eating Behavior" (2014). Dissertations . 235. https://irl.umsl.edu/dissertation/235

Since April 03, 2017

Included in

Psychology Commons

Advanced Search

  • Notify me via email or RSS
  • Collections
  • Disciplines

Participate

  • Submission Guidelines
  • Submit Research
  • University of Missouri, St. Louis
  • UMSL Library

Home | About | FAQ | My Account | Accessibility Statement

Privacy Copyright

IMAGES

  1. How to deal with Dissertation Writing Stress

    i'm stress eating over my dissertation

  2. Stress Eating: Understanding the Causes and Treatment

    i'm stress eating over my dissertation

  3. Top Recommendations to Lessen Dissertation Stress

    i'm stress eating over my dissertation

  4. Mentally Tired? Eating When You Are Stressed Will Make You FATTER

    i'm stress eating over my dissertation

  5. 21 Stress Eating Quotes To Inspire Willpower So You Stop Stress Eating

    i'm stress eating over my dissertation

  6. How to Stop Stress Eating and Why You Do It

    i'm stress eating over my dissertation

VIDEO

  1. Bro started stress eating 😂

  2. How to Stop Worrying About Eating Less and Start Loving the Food You Eat More

  3. EMOTIONAL EATING: क्या आप खुद को खाना खाने से रोक नहीं पाते

  4. How to CONTROL Your Food Cravings with Dr. Amy Shah

  5. Inside My Battle: Overcoming an Eating Disorder

  6. Eating Disorder

COMMENTS

  1. Impact of Stress Levels on Eating Behaviors among College Students

    Abstract. Weight gain is a common phenomenon among college students, especially those in their first year of university. Transitioning from high school to the college environment might increase perceived stress levels, thus affecting dietary behaviors and metabolism to promote overweight and obesity. The purpose of this study was to investigate ...

  2. All-nighters and self-doubt: learn from our dissertation disasters

    The dissertation was "a long, arduous process" for William Lloyd, a recent journalism graduate at Kingston University. "I caught the flu for the second time in my life, a week before it was ...

  3. Emotional Eating in College Students: Associations with ...

    Stress and other negative mood states influence eating behaviors in adults with and without eating disorder diagnoses [1,2,3].One key to understanding the relationship between stress and eating behaviors is emotional eating or eating in an effort to reduce stress and other aversive mood states [1, 4,5,6].There is evidence that general perceived stress, race-related stress, and recent traumatic ...

  4. (PDF) Stress and emotional eating: The mediating role of eating

    emotional eating. This study found that eating dysregulation mediated the association between stress. and emotional eating. Experience of stress was related to individuals' poorer capability of ...

  5. Unhealthy eating and academic stress: The moderating effect of eating

    Research has shown that most of the university students experience academic stress (Elias et al., 2011).In a study on the cardiovascular health of university students, Nguyen-Michel et al. (2006) found that more than half of students rate their stress level as high or very high. Exams have been identified as the primary source of academic stress for many university students (Chapell et al., 2005).

  6. Eating habits and stress: The relationship between social anxiety, fear

    Emotional eating, defined as eating in response to upsetting or intense emotions, as a form of regulating mood and emotion occurs regardless of previous clinically significant symptomology, and stress can be a catalyst for these behaviors (Cardi et al., 2015; Nyklicek et al., 2011). Women have a higher risk for disordered eating, and stress can

  7. The Relationship between Stress and Emotional Eating in Baccalaureate

    stress was 52% higher in 2021, compared with 2012. Emotional eating was 11% lower in 2021, compared with 2012. Continual high levels of stress can lead to physical and mental health. issues; therefore, it is important to take care of mental and physical well-being during stressful. times.

  8. Stress Eating and Health: Findings from MIDUS, a National Study of U.S

    The specific hypotheses of our study therefore were: Hypothesis 1 (H1). (a) Eating in response to stress will be associated with worse nondiabetic levels of fasting glucose, insulin, insulin resistance, and HbA 1c. (b) Waist circumference will mediate the relationships between using food in response to stress and fasting glucose, insulin ...

  9. Stress and Eating Behavior: A Daily Diary Study in Youngsters

    First, we hypothesize that emotional eating will be a proxy of maladaptive emotion regulation (based on Michopoulos et al., 2015).Second, we state that daily stress will be significantly associated with (1) the trajectories in desire to eat (2) the trajectories of hunger-eating and (3) the trajectories of snacking (Based on De Vriendt et al., 2009; Reichenberger et al., 2016; Vandewalle et al ...

  10. Mindful Eating and Perceived Stress in College Students

    The purpose of this study was to examine the relationships between students' perceived stress, their overall mindfulness, and mindful eating patterns. Gastrointestinal (GI) symptoms and bowel habits were also investigated to determine whether these factors were stress-related or influenced mindfulness behaviors.

  11. Five Tips for Managing Stress while Writing a Dissertation

    Tip 1: Create and Stick to a Routine. Establishing a routine is crucial in maintaining good mental health while writing a dissertation. A routine can help you manage stress and increase productivity by providing structure and predictability in your life (Gladding, 2020). Creating a routine can involve setting a schedule for writing, taking ...

  12. The relation between disordered eating, stress, and anxiety in first

    RELATION BETWEEN DISORDERED EATING, STRESS, AND ANXIETY 4 1. Eating in a discrete period of time (e.g. within any 2-hour period), an amount of food that is definitely larger than what most individuals would eat in a similar period of time under similar circumstances; 2. A sense of lack of control over eating during the episodes (e.g. a feeling

  13. Why stress causes people to overeat

    Epinephrine helps trigger the body's fight-or-flight response, a revved-up physiological state that temporarily puts eating on hold. But if stress persists, it's a different story. The adrenal glands release another hormone called cortisol, and cortisol increases appetite and may also ramp up motivation in general, including the motivation to eat.

  14. 5 Ways to Stop Stress Eating and Why You Should

    5 Ways to Handle Stress Eating. In order to get control of stress eating, you have to control your stress levels. The best way to deal with stress is to address current situations head-on and, at the same time, learn to be prepared to handle stressful situations in the future before both the problem and your eating behavior get out of hand.

  15. PDF Who eats their feelings, and who sweats them out?: A Dissertation

    That is, many people eat, smoke cigarettes, drink alcohol, use drugs, exercise, or even sleep to manage the experience and expression of their emotions (Gross, 1998). Specifically, they often do so to down-regulate (feel less) negative emotion and/or stress. For example, in one national survey, 27% of adults said they eat to manage stress,

  16. The Relation of Anxiety, Depression, and Stress to Binge Eating Behavior

    The main study hypotheses were supported, indicating a unique relation between anxiety. and binge eating, and between stress and binge eating, independent of the impact of. depression. Additionally, secondary analyses supported the role of cognitive avoidance as. a mediator in the relation of anxiety and binge eating.

  17. Examining the effects of stress on eating behaviour in children and

    In adults, stress has been found to initiate both increases and decreases in eating behaviours, and has been found to affect the types of foods consumed. However, there is a paucity of research on stress and eating amongst children and few studies have investigated the role of other factors such as positive and negative emotions. This thesis explored the impact that stress and emotions had on ...

  18. Stress Eating: Why It Happens and How To Stop

    1. Pause for mindfulness. Stress eating is often an automatic, knee-jerk reaction that you may not even recognize in the moment. It's both a habit and a learned response. " Mindfulness is an ...

  19. PDF Thesis Knowledge of And Attitudes Towards Eating Disorders of

    cases are even slightly overweight 6,9. The medical complications of BED are typically. overweight/obesity and the associated sequelae of issues associated with excess body weight, such as diabetes, hypertension, and cardiovascular disease 6. Eating disorders have a high rate of comorbidity with other mental disorders.

  20. Understanding Disordered Eating Attitudes and Patterns in University

    Dr. Travis Loof: Dr. Loof, I would like to thank you for being my faculty mentor and the chair of my thesis committee. With your support and assistance over the course of this past year, I was able to complete this project and make monumental strides in my education. Thank you for being such a great mentor over the course of my first research

  21. "The Relation of Anxiety, Depression, and Stress to Binge Eating ...

    The current study aimed to extend the literature by examining several psychological factors in relation to binge eating behavior. Specifically, the relation between binge eating behavior and three psychological factors, depression, anxiety and stress, were examined. Data were collected via online surveys from a community sample of men and women ...

  22. Dissertation Modeling Effects of Trauma and Stress on Disordered Eating

    I would like to thank my research and academic advisor, Dr. Michael F. Steger, for his insight, encouragement, and support throughout the course of my dissertation and graduate work. Dr. Steger has dedicated an incredible amount of time to my development as a researcher. While

  23. Advices on getting over self-doubt in writing my dissertation?

    I have been writing my dissertation for a few months now but even at that I am slow and not motivated. The more I write the more stress comes over me specially seeing that all the people from my intake of PhD are graduating and moving on to their jobs and that totally paralyzes my brain and motivation to go on for the rest of the day.