• Tools and Resources
  • Customer Services
  • Affective Science
  • Biological Foundations of Psychology
  • Clinical Psychology: Disorders and Therapies
  • Cognitive Psychology/Neuroscience
  • Developmental Psychology
  • Educational/School Psychology
  • Forensic Psychology
  • Health Psychology
  • History and Systems of Psychology
  • Individual Differences
  • Methods and Approaches in Psychology
  • Neuropsychology
  • Organizational and Institutional Psychology
  • Personality
  • Psychology and Other Disciplines
  • Social Psychology
  • Sports Psychology
  • Share This Facebook LinkedIn Twitter

Article contents

Identity development in adolescence and adulthood.

  • Jane Kroger Jane Kroger Department of Psychology, University of Tromsoe
  • https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.54
  • Published online: 27 February 2017

Psychoanalyst Erik Erikson was the first professional to describe and use the concept of ego identity in his writings on what constitutes healthy personality development for every individual over the course of the life span. Basic to Erikson’s view, as well as those of many later identity writers, is the understanding that identity enables one to move with purpose and direction in life, and with a sense of inner sameness and continuity over time and place. Erikson considered identity to be psychosocial in nature, formed by the intersection of individual biological and psychological capacities in combination with the opportunities and supports offered by one’s social context. Identity normally becomes a central issue of concern during adolescence, when decisions about future vocational, ideological, and relational issues need to be addressed; however, these key identity concerns often demand further reflection and revision during different phases of adult life as well. Identity, thus, is not something that one resolves once and for all at the end of adolescence, but rather identity may continue to evolve and change over the course of adult life too.

Following Erikson’s initial writings, subsequent theorists have laid different emphases on the role of the individual and the role of society in the identity formation process. One very popular elaboration of Erikson’s own writings on identity that retains a psychosocial focus is the identity status model of James Marcia. While Erikson had described one’s identity resolution as lying somewhere on a continuum between identity achievement and role confusion (and optimally located nearer the achievement end of the spectrum), Marcia defined four very different means by which one may approach identity-defining decisions: identity achievement (commitment following exploration), moratorium (exploration in process), foreclosure (commitment without exploration), and diffusion (no commitment with little or no exploration). These four approaches (or identity statuses) have, over many decades, been the focus of over 1,000 theoretical and research studies that have examined identity status antecedents, behavioral consequences, associated personality characteristics, patterns of interpersonal relations, and developmental forms of movement over time. A further field of study has focused on the implications for intervention that each identity status holds. Current research seeks both to refine the identity statuses and explore their dimensions further through narrative analysis.

  • identity status
  • identity formation
  • adolescence

Introduction

We know what we are, but not what we may be . Shakespeare, Hamlet

The question of what constitutes identity has been answered differently through different historical epochs and through different theoretical and empirical approaches to understanding identity’s form and functions. However, basic to all identity definitions is an attempt to understand the entity that, ideally, enables one to move with purpose and direction in life and with a sense of internal coherence and continuity over time and place. Despite the changing physique that aging inevitably brings and the changing environmental circumstances that one invariably encounters through life, a well-functioning identity enables one to experience feelings of personal meaning and well-being and to find satisfying and fulfilling engagements in one’s social context. The means by which one experiences a feeling of sameness in the midst of continual change is the focus of identity theory and research.

Historically, concerns with questions of identity are relatively recent. Baumeister and Muraven ( 1996 ) and Burkitt ( 2011 ) have noted how changes in Western society, specifically the degree to which society has dictated one’s adult roles, have varied enormously over time. Additional changes have occurred in the loosening of social guidelines, restrictions, and constraints, such that contemporary late adolescents experience almost unlimited freedom of choice in their assumption of adult roles and values. In Medieval times, adolescents and adults were prescribed an identity by society in a very direct manner. Social rank and the kinship networks into which one was born set one’s adult roles for life. In early modern times, wealth rather than kinship networks became the standard for self-definition. In the first half of the twentieth century , apprenticeship systems that prepared adolescents for one specific line of work were giving way to more liberal forms of education, thus preparing adolescents for a broad range of occupational pathways. A more liberal educational system, however, eventually required occupational choice in line with one’s own interests and capacities. In addition, many regions in the United States became more tolerant of diversity in attitudes and values, and gender roles became more fluid. Thus, by the middle of the twentieth century in the United States and many other Western nations, the burden of creating an adult identity was now falling largely on the shoulders of late adolescents themselves.

Into this twentieth century United States context came Erik Erikson, a German immigrant (escaping Hitler’s rise to power) and psychoanalyst, trained by Anna Freud. Erikson began his clinical work and writings on optimal personality development in the Boston area, focusing, in particular, on the concept of identity and identity crisis . As an immigrant, Erikson was acutely attuned to the role of the social context and its influence on individual personality development, and, as a psychoanalyst, he was also adept at understanding the roles of conscious as well as unconscious motivations, desires, and intentions, as well as biological drives on individual behavior.

Erikson ( 1963 ) first used the term “ego identity” to describe a central disturbance among some of his veteran patients returning from World War II with a diagnosis of “shell shock” (or currently, post-traumatic stress disorder), who seemed to be experiencing a loss of self-sameness and continuity in their lives:

What impressed me most was the loss in these men of a sense of identity. They knew who they were; they had a personal identity. But it was as if subjectively, their lives no longer hung together—and never would again. There was a central disturbance in what I then started to call ego identity. (Erikson, 1963 , p. 42)

Through identity’s absence in the lives of these young men, Erikson came to understand the tripartite nature of identity, that he believed to be comprised of biological, psychological, and social factors. It was often a particular moment in a soldier’s life history where soma, psyche, and society conspired to endanger identity foundations that necessitated clinical care. And, thus, it was through disruptions to individual identity that Erikson more clearly came to understand identity’s form and functions.

Erikson has often been referred to as “identity’s architect” (e.g., Friedman, 1999 ), and his initial writings on identity served as the springboard for many later theorists and researchers to examine further identity’s many dimensions. Erikson’s psychosocial approach will thus serve as the organizing framework for a review of research on identity development during adolescent and adult life.

Erikson’s Psychosocial Orientation

Erikson’s ( 1963 , 1968 ) understanding of identity views the phenomenon as a result of the mutual interaction of individual and context; while individual interests and capacities, wishes and desires draw individuals to particular contexts, those contexts, in turn, provide recognition (or not) of individual identity and are critical to its further development. Erikson stressed the important interactions among the biological, psychological, and social forces for optimal personality development. He suggested a series of eight psychosocial tasks over the course of the life span that follow an epigenetic principle, such that resolution to one task sets the foundation for all that follow. Identity vs. Role Confusion is the fifth psychosocial task that Erikson identified, becoming of primary importance during adolescence. Resolution to preceding tasks of Trust vs. Mistrust, Autonomy vs. Doubt and Shame, Initiative vs. Guilt, Industry vs. Inferiority are the foundations upon which one’s resolution to Identity vs. Role Confusion is based, according to Erikson; resolution to subsequent adult tasks of Intimacy vs. Role Confusion, Generativity vs. Stagnation, and Integrity vs. Despair all similarly depend upon resolution to the Identity vs. Role Confusion task of adolescence.

Erikson ( 1963 , 1968 ) postulated a number of key identity concepts that have served as foundations for much subsequent identity research. For Erikson, identity formation involves finding a meaningful identity direction on a continuum between identity attainment and role confusion . The process of identity formation requires identity exploration and commitment , the synthesis of childhood identifications into a new configuration, related to but different from, the sum of its parts. The identity formation process is extremely arduous for some, and the resolutions of a negative identity or identity foreclosure are two means by which the identity formation process can be bypassed. A negative identity involves identity choices based on roles and values that represent polar opposites of those espoused by one’s family and/or immediate community. Thus, the daughter of a Midwestern minister of religion runs away to become a prostitute in inner city Chicago. A foreclosed identity resolution also avoids the identity formation process by basing identity-defining choices on key identifications, mostly with parental values, without exploring potential alternatives.

Erikson ( 1963 , 1968 ) also proposed several further concepts for optimal identity development. A moratorium process, the active consideration and exploration of future possible identity-defining adult roles and values, was considered vital to optimal identity development. Erikson also became well known for his use of the term identity crisis , an acute period of questioning one’s own identity directions. And finally, Erikson stressed that while an initial resolution to the Identity vs. Role Confusion task often occurs during adolescence, identity is never resolved once and for all, but rather remains open to modifications and alterations throughout adult life. The strength of Erikson’s approach lies in its consideration of both individual and sociocultural factors and their mutual interaction in identity construction and development. Erikson’s model of identity development has wide applicability across cultural contexts and highlights the ongoing nature of identity development throughout adulthood. Weaknesses include his imprecise language, which at times makes operationalization of key concepts difficult, and his historically dated concepts regarding women’s identity development.

While other psychosocial models have evolved from Erikson’s original writings (e.g., Whitbourne’s [ 2002 ] identity processing theory, Berzonsky’s [ 2011 ] social cognitive identity styles, McAdams’s [ 2008 ] narrative approach), it is Erikson’s identity formation concepts, particularly those operationalized by Marcia ( 1966 ) (Marcia, Waterman, Matteson, Archer, & Orlofsky, 1993 ) that have generated an enormous volume of empirical research over past decades and will be the primary focus of subsequent sections of this article.

Erikson’s Psychosocial Approach and Marcia’s Identity Status Model

As a young Ph.D. student in clinical psychology, James Marcia was interested in Erikson’s writings but suspected that the process of identity formation during late adolescence to be somewhat more complicated than what Erikson ( 1963 ) had originally proposed. While Erikson had conceptualized an identity resolution as lying on a continuum between identity and role confusion, an entity that one had “more or less of,” Marcia proposed that there were four qualitatively different pathways by which late adolescents or young adults went about the process of forming an identity. Based on the presence or absence of exploration and commitment around several issues important to identity development during late adolescence, Marcia ( 1966 ; Marcia et al., 1993 ) developed a semi-structured Identity Status Interview to identify four identity pathways, or identity statuses, among late adolescent or young adult interviewees.

An individual in the identity achieved status had explored various identity-defining possibilities and had made commitments on his or her own terms, trying to match personal interests, talents, and values with those available in the environmental context. Equally committed to an identity direction was the foreclosed individual, who had formed an identity, but without undergoing an exploration process. This person’s identity had been acquired primarily through the process of identification—by assuming the identity choices of significant others without serious personal consideration of alternative possibilities. An individual in the moratorium identity status was very much in the process of identity exploration, seeking meaningful life directions but not yet making firm commitments and often experiencing considerable discomfort in the process. Someone in the diffusion identity status had similarly not made identity-defining commitments and was not attempting to do so.

Marcia et al.’s ( 1993 ) Identity Status Interview was designed to tap the areas (or domains) of occupation, political, religious, and sexual values that had been described by Erikson as key to the identity formation process. In Marcia’s view, however, the nature of the identity domain was not as critical to the assessment of identity status as was finding the identity-defining issues most salient to any given individual. Marcia suggested the use of clinical judgment in assigning a global identity status, the mode that seemed to best capture an adolescent’s identity formation process. It must be noted that Marcia and his colleagues (Marcia et al., 1993 ) have never attempted to capture all of the rich dimensions of identity outlined by Erikson through the Identity Status Interview; such a task would be unwieldy, if not impossible. Marcia does, however, build on Erikson’s concepts of identity exploration and comment to elaborate these identity dimensions in relation to those psychosocial roles and values identified by Erikson as key to the identity formation process of many late adolescents.

Subsequent to the original Identity Status Interview, several paper-and-pencil measures were developed to assess Marcia’s four identity statuses. One widely used measure has been the Extended Objective Measure of Ego Identity Status (EOM-EIS II), devised and revised through several versions by Adams and his colleagues (Adams, Bennion, & Huh, 1989 ; Adams & Ethier, 1999 ). This questionnaire measure enables identity status assessments in four ideological (occupation, religion, politics, philosophy of life) and four interpersonal domains (friendships, dating, gender roles, recreation/leisure), as well as providing a global rating.

Different dimensions of identity exploration and commitment processes have also been identified through several recent and expanded identity status models (Luyckx, Goossens, Soenens, & Beyers, 2006 ; Crocetti, Rubini, & Meeus, 2008 ). Luyckx and his colleagues differentiated two types of exploration (exploration in breadth and exploration in depth) and two types of commitment (commitment making and identification with commitment). Exploration in breadth is that moratorium process identified by Marcia, while exploration in depth describes the process of considering a commitment already made and how well it expresses one’s own identity. Commitment making refers to deciding an identity-defining direction, while identification with commitment describes the process of integrating one’s commitments into an internal sense of identity. Later, Luyckx and his colleagues (Luyckx, Schwartz, Berzonsky, Soenens, Vansteenkiste, Smits, et al., 2008 ) also identified a process of ruminative exploration.

Meeus and his colleagues (e.g., Crocetti, Rubini, & Meeus, 2008 ) also identified three identity processes: commitment, exploration in depth, and reconsideration of commitments. Commitment here refers to the dimensions of commitment making and identification with commitment in the Luyckx, Goossens, Soenens, and Beyers ( 2006 ) model; exploration in depth corresponds to that dimension in the Luyckx model. Reconsideration of commitment refers to one’s willingness to replace current commitments with new ones. In this model, commitment and reconsideration reflect identity certainty and uncertainty, respectively, in the identity formation process.

Through cluster analysis, these two groups of researchers have extracted clusters that match all of Marcia’s original identity statuses. In addition, Luyckx and his colleagues (Luyckx, Goossens, Soenens, Beyers, & Vansteenkiste, 2005 ) identified two types of diffusion—troubled and carefree—while Meeus, van de Schoot, Keijsers, Schwartz, and Branje ( 2010 ) found two types of moratoriums—classical (where the individual exhibits anxiety and depression in the identity exploration process) and searching (where new commitments are considered without discarding present commitments). Work has now begun to explore the identity formation process during adolescence and young adulthood with these refined identity statuses, which hold interesting implications for understanding both adaptive and non-adaptive identity development.

Over the time since Marcia’s initial studies, the identity statuses have been examined in relation to personality and behavioral correlates, relationship styles, and developmental patterns of change over time. Most of the studies reviewed in subsequent sections address some aspect of identity development during adolescence or young adulthood; a later section will focus on identity development research during adulthood. It must be further noted that discussion of identity statuses here will be limited to general (or global) identity and its relationship to associated variables.

Personality and Behavioral Correlates of the Identity Statuses

Work utilizing Marcia’s original identity status model, as well as its more recent refinements, have focused on personality and behavioral variables associated with each identity status in order to help validate the model; such studies have produced some reasonably consistent results over time. In terms of personality variables associated with the identity statuses, Kroger and her colleagues (e.g., Martinussen & Kroger, 2013 ) have produced a series of findings utilizing techniques of meta-analysis. Meta-analysis is a “study of studies,” using statistical procedures to examine (sometimes contradictory) results from different individual studies addressing comparable themes over time. Results from such meta-analytic studies allow greater confidence in results than a narrative review of individual studies can provide. The personality variables of self-esteem, anxiety, locus of control, authoritarianism, moral reasoning, and ego development and their relations to identity status have attracted sufficient studies for meta-analyses to be undertaken and are described in the sections that follow. While a number of other personality variables have also been examined in identity status studies over the past decades, their numbers have been insufficient to enable meta-analytic studies.

An initial database for all studies included in the meta-analytic work described in the following sections was comprised of some 565 English-language studies (287 journal publications and 278 doctoral dissertations) identified from PsycInfo, ERIC, Sociological Abstracts, and Dissertation Abstracts International databases, using the following search terms: identity and Marcia, identity and Marcia’s, and ego identity. Cohen’s ( 1988 ) criteria were used to define small, medium, and large effect sizes. In some of the meta-analyses that follow, different methods were used to assess identity status (categorical ratings of identity status and scale measures of identity status). Separate meta-analyses had to be undertaken for studies utilizing each of these two types of identity status assessments for statistical reasons.

Self-Esteem

Ryeng, Kroger, and Martinussen ( 2013a ) undertook meta-analytic studies of the relationship between identity status and global self-esteem. A total of twelve studies with 1,124 participants provided the data for these studies. The achieved identity status was the only status to have a positive correlation with self-esteem ( r = .35), considered to be moderate in effect size. Mean correlations between self-esteem and the moratorium, foreclosure, and diffusion statuses were all negative (−.23, −.23, and −.20, respectively) and considered small to moderate in effect size. All of these correlations were significantly different from zero, based on their confidence intervals. When identity status was assessed categorically, there was no difference in effect size between achievements and foreclosures on self-esteem measures. The effect size for the foreclosure-diffusion comparison ( g̅ = −0.19) was small to medium and also significant. Remaining comparisons evidenced small effect size differences in self-esteem scores. Findings here were mixed, as previous research had also produced mixed results on the question of whether foreclosure self-esteem scores would be lower than or similar to those of the identity achieved. Here, results show that only the achieved status (when the identity statuses were measured by continuous scales) produced a moderately positive correlation with self-esteem, while there was no difference in effect sizes between the achieved and foreclosed identity status when studies assessing identity status categorically were analyzed. Thus, the relationship between identity status and self-esteem may depend upon how identity status is measured.

Lillevoll, Kroger, and Martinussen ( 2013a ) examined the relationship between identity status and generalized anxiety through meta-analysis. Twelve studies involving 2,104 participants provided data for this investigation. Effect size differences in anxiety scores for moratoriums compared with foreclosures ( g̅ = 0.39) and for the foreclosure–diffusion comparison ( g̅ = −0.40) were small to moderate. Additionally the confidence intervals for both of these effect sizes did not contain zero, indicating a significant result. A significant moderate effect size ( g̅ = 0.46) was also found in the achievement–foreclosure comparison, but for men only. As predicted, foreclosures had lower anxiety scores compared with all other identity statuses except the achievement women. While it was predicted that those in the achievement identity status would have lower anxiety scores than those in moratorium and diffusion statuses, a small but significant effect size difference was found for the achievement–moratorium comparison only ( g̅ = −0.22). Thus, the moratoriums showed higher generalized anxiety scores than foreclosures, who, in turn, showed lower anxiety scores than the diffusions and male achievements. It appears that unexamined identity commitments undertaken by the foreclosures provided relief from the anxieties and uncertainties of uncommitted identity directions experienced by the moratoriums and diffusions.

Locus of Control

Lillevoll, Kroger, and Martinussen ( 2013b ) examined the relationship between identity status and locus of control. Some five studies with a total of 711 participants provided data for this study. A positive correlation between identity achievement and internal locus of control ( r = .26) and a negative correlation between identity achievement and external locus of control ( r = −.17) was found; these effect sizes are considered small to medium. The moratorium identity status was negatively correlated with internal locus of control ( r = −.17) and positively with an external locus of control ( r = .17), both considered small to medium effect sizes. The foreclosure status was negatively correlated the internal locus of control ( r = −.12) and positively with external locus of control ( r = .19), both considered small to medium effect sizes. The diffusions’ status was negatively correlated with internal locus of control ( r = −.15) and positively with external locus of control ( r = .23), both considered small to medium effect sizes. Apart from the moratorium findings, which were anticipated to reflect an internal locus of control, all other results were in expected directions. It appears that the ability to undertake identity explorations on one’s own terms by the identity achieved is associated with an internal locus of control. Moratorium, foreclosure, and diffusion statuses are associated with an external locus of control.

Authoritarianism

The relationship between identity status and authoritarianism was investigated by Ryeng, Kroger, and Martinussen ( 2013b ) through meta-analysis. Some nine studies involving 861 participants provided data for this study. The mean difference between authoritarianism scores for the achievement—foreclosure comparison ( g̅ = −0.79) was large in terms of Cohen’s criteria and significant. The mean difference in authoritarianism scores for the moratorium–foreclosure comparison ( g̅ = −0.67) was medium and significant, while the mean difference in authoritarianism scores for the foreclosure and diffusion identity statuses was medium ( g̅ = 0.42) and significant. Other comparisons were relatively small and not significant. That the foreclosures scored higher on authoritarianism than all other identity statuses is consistent with expectations. Foreclosures often base their identity commitments on their identifications with significant others, rather than exploring identity options on their own terms; thus, the rigidity and intolerance of authoritarian attitudes seem to characterize the terms of their identity commitments, in contrast to the more flexible commitments of the identity achieved or moratoriums in the process of finding their own identity directions.

Ego Development

Jespersen, Kroger, and Martinussen ( 2013a ) examined studies utilizing Loevinger’s ( 1976 ) measure of ego development in relation to the identity statuses through meta-analysis. Eleven studies involving 943 participants provided data for this investigation. Odds ratios (OR) were used to examine frequency distributions of the categorical data. Results of correlational studies showed a moderate, positive relationship between ego development and identity status ( r = .35), which was significant. Results from categorical assessments of identity status also showed a strong relationship between identity status and ego development (mean OR = 3.02). This finding means that the odds of being in a postconformist level of ego development were three times greater for those high in identity statuses (achievement and moratorium) compared with those in the low identity statuses (foreclosure and diffusion). The study also found a moderate relationship between identity achievement and ego development (mean OR = 2.15), meaning that the odds of being in a postconformist level of ego development were over two times greater for those in the identity achievement status than remaining identity statuses. However, no relationship was found between the foreclosed/nonforeclosed identity statuses and the conformist/nonconformist levels of ego development, contrary to prediction (mean OR = 1.31). While results indicate a strong likelihood of being in a post-conformist level of ego development for the identity achieved and moratoriums, as one would predict, it is somewhat surprising that the foreclosure status was not associated with conventional levels of ego development. This lack of association requires further investigation.

Moral Reasoning

A meta-analysis of moral reasoning stages (using Kohlberg’s [ 1976 ] stages in relation to the identity statuses) was also undertaken by Jespersen, Kroger, and Martinussen ( 2013b ). Some ten studies involving 884 participants provided data appropriate for this study. Results showed a small positive mean correlation (.15) between identity status and moral reasoning development, which was significant. Results from categorical assessments of both measures indicated a strong relationship between high identity status (achievement and moratorium) and postconventional levels of moral reasoning (mean OR = 4.57). This result means that the odds of being in the postconventional level of moral reasoning are about four and a half times greater for the high identity status group (achievement and moratorium) than the low (foreclosure and diffusion) group. A strong relationship was also found between the achieved identity status and the postconventional level of moral reasoning (mean OR = 8.85), meaning that the odds of being in a postconventional level of moral reasoning were almost nine times greater for the identity achieved than for other identity statuses. However, no significant relationship appeared for the foreclosed/nonforeclosed identity statuses and the conventional/nonconventional levels of moral reasoning, contrary to prediction. While a meaningful relationship was found between postconventional stages of moral reasoning and the moratorium and achievement identity statuses, it is again surprising that no relationship appeared for the foreclosed identity status and conventional levels of moral reasoning. This finding warrants further investigation.

Additional Personality and Behavioral Variables

A number of additional personality and behavioral variables have been explored in relation to the identity statuses, but no further meta-analyses have yet been undertaken. With regard to the newer, more refined measures of identity status, some additional personality and behavioral associations have been noted. Luyckx et al. ( 2008 ) found ruminative exploration related to identity distress and low self-esteem, while exploration in breadth and depth were positively related to self-reflection. Furthermore, commitment-making (particularly identification with commitment) was associated with high self-esteem, high academic and social adjustment, as well as with low depressive symptoms. Crocetti et al. ( 2008 ) similarly found strong, positive associations between commitment and self-concept clarity, in addition to strong negative associations between in-depth exploration and reconsideration of commitment with self-reflection. Emotional stability was strongly associated with commitment and negatively with in-depth exploration.

Recent work has performed cluster analyses on the exploration and commitment variables, finding four clusters replicating Marcia’s four identity statuses (with the diffusion status including carefree and diffuse diffusions) and an undifferentiated status (Schwartz et al., 2011 ). In terms of psychosocial functioning, achievements were significantly higher than carefree diffusions on a measure of self-esteem; diffusions, in turn, were significantly lower than all other identity statuses on this variable. On a measure of internal locus of control, achievements and moratoriums were significantly higher and carefree diffusions significantly lower than all other identity statuses. On psychological well-being, identity achievements scored significantly higher and carefree diffusions significantly lower than all other identity status groups. For general anxiety, moratoriums and the two diffusion groups scored significantly higher than achievement and foreclosure groups, while the moratoriums scored significantly higher than foreclosures and the two diffusions groups on depression. These findings are generally in line with findings of earlier studies using Marcia’s original model.

Further behavioral studies in relation to the identity statuses have consistently found the identity diffusion status to be related to psychosocial problem behaviors. Delinquent behavior (e.g., Jessor, Turbin, Costa, Dong, Zhang, & Wang, 2003 ; Schwartz, Pantin, Prado, Sullivan, & Szapocznik, 2005 ), substance abuse (e.g., Jones & Hartmann, 1988 ; Laghi, Baiocco, Longiro, & Baumgartner, 2013 ), risky behaviors (e.g., unsafe sex, Hernandez & DiClemente, 1992 ), social, physical aggression, and rule-breaking (carefree diffusions, Schwartz et al., 2011 ), and procrastination (Shanahan & Pychyl, 2006 ) have all been linked with the identity diffusion status. By contrast, the identity achieved have demonstrated a low prevalence of all preceding problem behaviors, coupled with high levels of agency or self-direction and commitment making (e.g., Schwartz et al., 2011 ; Shanahan & Pychyl, 2006 ). Moratoriums have also scored relatively high on levels of social and physical aggression, although they have also scored high on a number of psychosocial measures of well-being (e.g., Schwartz et al., 2011 ).

Relationships and the Identity Statuses

While a number of relational issues have been explored in identity status research (e.g., parental attitudes toward childrearing, family styles of communication, and friendship styles), to date, meta-analyses have been undertaken to examine identity status only in relation to attachment patterns and intimacy or romantic relationships.

Bartholomew and Horowitz ( 1991 ) have proposed that one’s very unique attachment history and subsequent working models of attachment lead to one of four different adolescent/adult attachment styles, or patterns of relating to significant others; these attachment styles become activated particularly in times of stress. S ecurely attached individuals are at ease in becoming close to others and do not worry about being abandoned or having someone become too close to them. Furthermore, they are interdependent—comfortable depending on others and having others depend on them. Those using the avoidant attachment style find it difficult to trust and depend on others and are uncomfortable in becoming too emotionally close. The preoccupied (anxious/ambivalent) attachment group wants to be close to others but worries that others will not reciprocate and will abandon them, while the fearful attachment group wants to be emotionally close to others but are too frightened of being hurt to realize this desire.

These varied styles of attachment have been examined in relation to Marcia’s identity statuses among adolescents and young adults in a number of studies over the past decades, and recent meta-analytic work has explored patterns of findings across studies (Årseth, Kroger, Martinussen, & Marcia, 2009 ). From the large database of 565 identity status studies described earlier, some 14 had data suitable for meta-analysis (a full description of the database can be found in Martinussen & Kroger, 2013 ). A total of 2,329 participants were involved in this investigation. Weak to moderate correlations were found between identity status and attachment style when scale measures were used to assess each variable; the highest mean correlations were between the secure attachment style and identity achievement ( r = .21) as well as identity diffusion ( r = −.23). (Cohen, 1988 , regarded a correlation of .30 as moderate and .10 as weak.) The diffusion status was also weakly to moderately positively correlated with the fearful attachment style ( r = .19). Among categorical assessments of identity status and attachment style, results suggest there are real differences between the identity achieved and foreclosed as well as diffusion identity statuses, with the identity achieved far more likely to be securely attached than foreclosed or diffusion statuses. Data from these studies suggests that one’s relational experiences do have some links to one’s identity status.

According to Erikson’s ( 1963 , 1968 ) epigenetic principle, resolution to the task of Identity vs. Intimacy should set the foundation for resolution to the task of Intimacy vs. Isolation during late adolescence and young adulthood. In Erikson’s ( 1968 ) view, true intimacy involves mutuality and commitment, an acceptance of another with all of his or her strengths and weaknesses in an interdependent, sexual relationship. Erikson ( 1968 ) believed that genuine intimacy requires a sense of identity to be firmly in place, or the relationship becomes merely a tool to help resolve identity concerns for each partner. However, Erikson was unclear about the potential for gender differences in his theory, and a number of feminist writers (e.g., Gilligan, 1982 ) have stressed the importance of relationship issues for women to the identity formation process. Literature examining the relationship between identity and intimacy statuses for late adolescent and young adult men and women has often produced conflicting results.

Thus, a meta-analysis of the relationship between identity status and intimacy for men and women was undertaken by Årseth, Kroger, Martinussen, and Marcia ( 2009 ). Some 21 studies with a total of 1,983 participants were included in meta-analyses here. For studies utilizing scale measures of intimacy, results indicated a low to moderate effect size for men ( g̅ = .35) and women ( g̅ = .30) considered separately, as well as for the total group ( g̅ = .40). All results were significant and indicate that high identity status individuals (achievement and moratorium) scored higher on scale measures of intimacy than low identity status individuals (foreclosures and diffusions). For categorical assessments of identity and intimacy, the picture was somewhat more complex. Among men, the mean odds ratio of having both a high identity and high intimacy status was very high at 22.09, while for women the mean odds ratio was 2.61. In terms of percentages, some 69% of high identity status men were also high in intimacy, while only 23% of low identity status men were high in intimacy. Erikson’s epigenetic principle thus finds strong support among men. Among women, while 65% of high identity status women were also high in intimacy status, some 46% of low identity status women were also high in intimacy status. Thus, the low identity status women were almost equally distributed over high and low intimacy status groups. These results indicate Erikson’s epigenetic principle also was present for a large proportion of women sampled; however, the relationship was significantly stronger for men than women (p < .001), and reasons for this gender difference require further investigation.

Identity Status Change from Adolescence Through Adulthood

Erikson ( 1963 , 1968 ) had proposed that while making initial identity resolutions was a key developmental task of adolescence, identity remained malleable, open to further changes throughout adult life. Similarly, the identity status literature that has pointed to different patterns of movement during young, middle, and late adolescence clearly shows that identity will continue to meet challenges and, for some, the need for revision throughout adulthood. What are the most prevalent patterns of identity status change over the course of adolescent and adult life, and what are the key events primarily associated with these changes?

A number of studies addressing identity status changes over time have now been undertaken, and a series of meta-analytic investigations are perhaps the most effective means of summarizing common patterns of movement and stability in the identity status literature. Kroger, Martinussen, and Marcia ( 2010 ) investigated some 72 of 124 identity studies that contained developmental information from the larger database of 565 English-language identity status studies described earlier. Movement patterns were investigated in several ways.

When movements over approximately three years of late adolescence and young adulthood were examined longitudinally from data that assessed identity status in categorical terms, the mean proportion of adolescents making progressive identity status changes (D–F, D–M, D–A, F–M, F–A, and M–A) was .36, compared with .15 who made regressive changes (A–M, A–F, A–D, M–F, M–D, and F–D) and .49 who remained stable (A–A, M–M, F–F, D–D) over this time period. It is interesting that the mean proportion of those remaining stable in identity status was so high, especially during the time of late adolescence that Erikson ( 1968 ) has identified as central to the identity formation process. As anticipated, the highest mean proportions of progressive movements were from M–A (.46), F–A (.22), and F–M (.22). The highest mean proportions of those remaining stable were the committed identity achieved (.66) and the foreclosed (.53) statuses. The highest mean proportions of those making regressive movements were from A–F (.17) and M–F (.17).

For cross-sectional studies assessing identity status in categorical terms, the mean proportion of identity achievements increased steadily through the high school years, dropped upon university entry and increased to .34 by age 22 years. It was not until the 30–36 year age group that about half of the participants were rated identity achieved (.47). The mean proportion of moratoriums rose fairly steadily to age 19 years, which peaked at .42 and declined fairly steadily thereafter through the 30–36 year age span. The mean proportion of foreclosures dropped fairly steadily to a low at age 19 years of .12, but then showed and up and down movement throughout remaining ages to .17 in the 30–36 year age group. The mean proportion of diffusions declined fairly steadily from age 14–20 years of age (from .36 to .21), but by age 21 years, the diffusions rose again to .26 and showed up and down movement until the final 30–36 year age span (.14).

For cross-sectional studies using continuous measures of identity status, it was anticipated that achievement and moratorium scores would increase across age groups and foreclosure and diffusion scores would decrease over time. Studies here were based on data for early and mid-adolescents. The anticipated patterns were found, but all effect sizes were small. It may be that more pronounced identity status changes occur during and beyond late adolescence.

Additional studies of identity status change through middle and later adulthood years not included in meta-analyses have also generally found slow, progressive identity status movements over time. Fadjukoff, Pulkkinen, and Kokko ( 2016 ) analyzed identity status longitudinally in a Finnish sample of men and women drawn from the general population. Identity status was assessed at ages 27, 36, 42, and 50 years. Movement towards identity achievement was predominant on the overall measure of identity status, with women typically reaching identity achievement earlier than men. In a narrative analysis of identity pathways among women assessed from late adolescence through mid-life, Josselson ( 1996 ) found a diversity of identity pathways, with achievement and foreclosure pathways tending to be the most stable over time. Carlsson, Wängqvist, and Frisén ( 2015 ) have also examined identity status change and stability in a longitudinal study of young adults at ages 25 and 29 years in Sweden. Half of participants were coded in the same identity status at Times 1 and 2, while half who changed did so in a progressive direction. Additional identity processes of how people approach life-changing situations, the extent to which they continue to engage in meaning-making, and how they continue to develop their personal life directions were explored through narrative methods among foreclosed and achieved participants. Identity achievement was associated with continued identity development over time, while patterns for ongoing development among foreclosures were more mixed. McLean and Pasupathi ( 2012 ) have made a plea for the use of narrative methods that examine reconstructions of past events to supplement current understandings of the exploration and commitment processes involved on ongoing identity development throughout the life span. Additional identity processes may usefully be identified through such means.

Events Associated with Identity Status Change

An issue that researchers have been exploring over several decades is the question of what kinds of circumstances are associated with identity status change and, conversely, what circumstances are linked with identity status stability. Some hints have appeared in related literatures. For example, Helson and Roberts ( 1994 ) found that some optimal level of “accommodative challenge” or life stimulation is critical for adult ego development (referring to Loevinger’s, 1976 , model of ego development). Accommodative challenge is a circumstance or event that involves either a positive or negative disruption to one’s life. It may be that such life challenges are important to ongoing identity development over time as well.

Anthis and colleagues (Anthis, 2002 , 2011 ; Anthis & La Voie, 2006 ) have conducted several investigations into life events associated with identity exploration and commitment. In her “calamity theory of growth” model, Anthis ( 2002 ) has found stressful life events, such as divorce or job loss, to be associated with increased levels of identity exploration and decreases in identity commitments. She has also found increased levels of identity exploration to be associated with a “readiness for change” measure (Anthis & La Voie, 2006 ). Anthis suggests investigating how optimal levels of perceived conflict interact with other factors for different cohorts of people in exploring the role that life events may play in ongoing identity development during adulthood.

Additionally, Kunnen ( 2006 , 2010 ) asks if conflict may be the driver of identity change. In a study of freshman university students, she found that students who experienced a conflict in their career goals increased identity exploratory activity and also manifested a decrease in the strength of their present commitments. Furthermore, those experiencing conflict perceived more change in their commitments as compared to nonconflicted students. The types and levels of perceived identity conflict and the mechanisms by which conflict may stimulate or impair ongoing identity development are in need of further study. Lilgendahl’s ( 2015 ) narrative work reiterates the value of negative events and their potential for psychological growth during midlife, while events that are understood as positive are key to the formation of identity commitments during young adulthood.

Identity Development in Adulthood

Research into ongoing identity development during adulthood has taken several forms. Some researchers have attempted to understand the relationship between resolution to identity issues during late adolescence or young adulthood and the Eriksonian psychosocial tasks of adulthood: Intimacy vs. Isolation (young adulthood), Generativity vs. Stagnation (middle adulthood), and Integrity vs. Despair (late adulthood). Others have attempted to examine selected issues of identity during these specific adult life phases and whether or not identity cohesion and stability increase with age over the course of adulthood. The following brief overview presents some selected findings from these strands of identity research during various phases of adult life.

According to Erikson’s ( 1963 , 1968 ) epigenetic principle, resolutions to earlier psychosocial tasks will impact resolutions to all subsequent ones. Research to date has generally supported this proposal, with some caveats for the relationship between identity and intimacy, described in meta-analytic studies in a preceding section. The relationships among identity, generativity, and integrity have only recently become a focus of research attention, and they present important opportunities for future investigations. Beaumont and Pratt ( 2011 ) have examined links among Berzonsky’s ( 2011 ) identity styles, Intimacy vs. Isolation, and Generativity vs. Stagnation in samples of young and midlife adults. They found that the informational style (associated with identity achievement) was linked with both the capacity for intimacy and generativity, while the diffuse–avoidant style (associated with identity diffusion) was negatively linked with both intimacy and generativity. The normative identity style (associated with the foreclosure identity status) also positively predicted resolution to intimacy and generativity tasks of adulthood. Pulkkinen, Lyyra, Fadjukoff, and Kokko ( 2012 ) obtained longitudinal data from Finnish adults at ages 27, 36, 42, and 50 years on measures including parental identity, general identity, generativity, and integrity. Generativity scores (as well as scores for psychological and social well-being) were highest if parental identity was achieved by age 42. On a cross-sectional basis, Hearn, Saulnier, Strayer, Glenham, Koopman, and Marcia ( 2012 ) examined the relationship between identity status and a measure of integrity status. A significant relationship was found, with some 86% of integrated persons rated as identity achieved, while no despairing persons were. Those in the non-exploring integrity status (in which questions of personal life meanings were unexplored), the pseudo-integrated integrity status (in which the world was understood in terms of simplistic templates or clichéd meanings), and the despairing integrity status were most frequently in the foreclosed identity status. Hannah, Domino, Figueredo, and Hendrickson ( 1996 ) explored predictors of Integrity vs. Despair in a sample of later life adults, finding the most predictive and parsimonious variables to be trust, autonomy, identity, and intimacy, with no meaningful gender differences. Thus, Erikson’s epigenetic principle has found considerable support over time and illustrates the important role that identity resolution plays to the resolution of subsequent psychosocial tasks during adulthood.

While Erikson ( 1963 , 1968 ) had postulated the ongoing nature of identity development throughout adulthood, and Stephen, Fraser, and Marcia ( 1992 ) had first proposed the likelihood of ongoing moratorium–achievement–moratorium–achievement cycles in adult identity development, there have been relatively few efforts to examine the nature of change and continuity in identity development over the course of adulthood. While some early research has estimated the likelihood of a midlife identity crisis to be about 10% (e.g., Brim, 1992 ), recent work has pointed to ongoing times of identity crisis (or revision) during the later adult years as well (Robinson & Stell, 2015 ). Experiences of well-being have been examined in relation to adult psychosocial stage resolutions in the Rochester Adult Longitudinal Study (Sneed, Whitbourne, Schwartz, & Huang, 2011 ), where scores on both identity and intimacy measures in early and middle adulthood predicted midlife feelings of satisfaction and well-being. A sense of coherence and life satisfaction in later adult years has been fully mediated by resolution to Integrity vs. Despair (Dezutter, Wiesmann, Apers, & Luyckx, 2013 ). Much remains to be learned about ongoing identity development in the adulthood years, and the relation of identity to subsequent psychosocial tasks and additional personality variables.

What the Identity Statuses Mean

Through the decades since Marcia ( 1966 ) developed his identity status model, there has been considerable discussion in the literature about what the identity statuses actually mean and how best to assess them. Marcia ( 1980 ) considers identity to be a structure for organizing individual conscious and unconscious wishes, interests, skills, and talents within the framework of one’s biology and cultural context. His identity status model was intended to reflect the movement through Erikson’s ( 1963 , 1968 ) identity formation process, from an identity based on identifications (foreclosure status), through an exploration (moratorium) process, to a new configuration, based on but different from the sum of its identificatory elements (achievement).

In considering the question of what it is that actually changes in an identity status transition, Kroger ( 2003 ) has suggested that qualitatively different forms of ego organization underlie each of Marcia’s identity statuses. However, after an initial identity has formed, further use of the identity status model during adult life begs the question of what the identity statuses actually mean when applied to adults. While new identity-defining decisions may occur in adult life, is there an actual underlying structural change of identity? There may or may not be. There may actually be new or additional structures of ego organization that underlie the identity achievement status of adulthood, and future research could fruitfully explore this issue. Lile ( 2013 , 2015 ) considers structural identity boundaries for each of the identity statuses and offers some empirical support for a structural model of identity that underlies the identity statuses. Identity status research in adulthood should carefully consider the meaning that the identity statuses may hold when applied to a life phase beyond that for which they were originally developed.

Conclusions

Historically, the task of identity formation is a relatively recent phenomenon. Erikson ( 1963 , 1968 ) first used the identity concept in his clinical writings to describe that entity that seemed to be lacking in the lives of young men returning from combat in World War II. From Erikson’s early writings, several broad approaches to identity theory and research have emerged, laying differential emphasis on the psychosocial, phenomenological, and the contextual nature of identity. This article has reviewed some of the writings and research that have sprung from the identity status model of James Marcia ( 1966 , 1980 ). This review has documented meta-analytic work covering the associations of Marcia’s four identity statuses with various personality, relational, and behavioral variables, as well as documenting the most common patterns of identity status change and stability during adolescence and adulthood. The review has also documented the role that resolution to questions of identity plays in resolutions to ongoing psychosocial tasks of adulthood.

Further identity research could fruitfully explore both the meaning of the identity statuses in ongoing adult identity development as well as the processes and contents of identity changes during adult life. The role of regression in adolescent and adult identity development is poorly understand, occurring more frequently than can be predicted by chance alone (see Kroger et al., 2010 ). Understanding what kinds of regression there may be and whether or not specific types of regression are vital to ongoing adult identity development are important avenues for further identity research. And though identity concerns of adolescence have many parallels to identity issues of later adulthood, very little identity-related theory and research has been undertaken with older adults. (For example, individuals in both phases of the life span must adjust to important biological changes, deal with philosophical questions of life’s meanings, and readjustment to changing demands from social contexts.) It is hoped that this article will present a foundation upon which future psychosocial research into the process and contents of identity development from adolescence through adulthood can take place.

Further Reading

  • Berman, S. L. , & Montgomery, M. J. (Guest Eds.). (2014). Problematic identity processes: the role of identity distress [Special issue]. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 14 , 241–312.
  • Côté, J. E. (2006). Identity studies: How close are we to developing a social science of identity?—An appraisal of the field. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 6 , 3–25.
  • Erikson, E. H. (Ed.). (1978). Adulthood . New York: W. W. Norton.
  • Erikson, E. H. , & Erikson, J. M. (1997). The life cycle completed . New York: W. W. Norton.
  • Josselson, R. , & Flum, H. (2015). Identity status: On refinding the people. In K. C. McLean & M. Syed (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of identity development (pp. 132–146). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Kroger, J. (2007). Identity development: Adolescence through adulthood . Newbury Park, CA: SAGE.
  • Kroger, J. , & Marcia, J. E. (2011). The identity statuses: Origins, meanings, and interpretations. In S. J. Schwartz , K. Luyckx , & V. L. Vignoles (Eds.), Handbook of identity theory and research (pp. 31–53). New York: Springer.
  • Luyckx, K. , Schwartz, S. J. , Goossens, L. , Beyers, W. , & Missotten, L. (2011). Processes of personal identity formation and evaluation. In S. J. Schwartz , K. Luyckx , & V. L. Vignoles (Eds.), Handbook of identity theory and research, Vol. 1, Structures and Processes (pp. 77–98). New York: Springer.
  • Marcia, J. E. (2010). Life transitions and stress in the context of psychosocial development. In T. W. Miller (Ed.), Handbook of stressful transitions across the lifespan (pp. 19–34). New York: Springer.
  • Marcia, J. E. (2014). From industry to integrity. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 14 , 165–176.
  • Adams, G. R. , Bennion, L. , & Huh, K. (1989). Objective measure of ego identity status: A reference manual . Ontario, Canada: University of Guelph.
  • Adams, G. R. , & Ethier, J. L. (1999). The objective measure of ego identity status: A manual on theory and test construction . Ontario, Canada: University of Guelph.
  • Anthis, K. (2002). On the calamity theory of growth: The relationship between stressful life events and changes in identity over time. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 2 , 229–240.
  • Anthis, K. (2011). The role of conflict in continuity and change: Life events associated with identity development in racially and ethnically diverse women. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 11 , 333–347.
  • Anthis, K. , & La Voie, J. (2006). Readiness to change: A longitudinal study of changes in adult identity. Journal of Research in Personality, 40 , 209–219.
  • Årseth, A. K. , Kroger, J. , Martinussen, M. , & Marcia, J. E. (2009). Meta-analytic studies of the relational issues of attachment and intimacy. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 9 , 1–32.
  • Bartholomew, K. , & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61 , 226–244.
  • Baumeister, R. F. , & Muraven, M. (1996). Identity as adaptation to social, cultural, and historical context. Journal of Adolescence, 19 , 405–416.
  • Beaumont, S. L. , & Pratt, M. M. (2011). Identity processing styles and psychosocial balance during early and middle adulthood: The role of identity in intimacy and generativity. Journal of Adult Development, 18 , 1172–1183.
  • Berzonsky, M. D. (2011). A social-cognitive perspective on identity construction. In S. J. Schwartz , K. Luyckx , & V. L. Vignoles (Eds.), Handbook of identity theory and research: Structures and processes, Vol. 1 (pp. 55–75). New York: Springer.
  • Brim, O. G. (1992). Ambition . New York: Basic Books.
  • Burkitt, I. (2011). Identity construction in sociohistorical context. In S. J. Schwartz , K. Luyckx , & V. L. Vignoles (Eds.), Handbook of identity theory and research: Structures and processes, Vol. 1 (pp. 267–283). New York: Springer.
  • Carlsson, J. , Wängqvist, M. , & Frisén, A. (2015). Identity development in the late twenties: A never ending story. Developmental Psychology, 51 , 334–345.
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2d ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Crocetti, E. , Rubini, M. , & Meeus, W. (2008). Capturing the dynamics of identity formation in various ethnic groups: Development and validation of a three-dimensional model. Journal of Adolescence, 31 , 207–222.
  • Dezutter, J. , Wiesmann, U. , Apers, S. , & Luyckx, K. (2013). Sense of coherence, depressive feelings, and life satisfaction in older persons: A closer look at the role of integrity and despair . Aging and Mental Health, 17 , 839–843.
  • Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society (2d ed.). New York: W. W. Norton.
  • Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity, youth and crisis . New York: W. W. Norton.
  • Fadjukoff, P. , Pulkkinen, L. , & Kokko, K. (2016). Identity formation in adulthood: A longitudinal study from age 27 to 50. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 16 , 8–23.
  • Friedman, L. J. (1999). Identity’s architect: A biography of Erik Erikson . New York: Scribner.
  • Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice. Psychological theory and women’s development . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Hannah, M. T. , Domino, G. , Figueredo, A. J. , & Hendrickson, R. (1996). The prediction of ego integrity in older persons. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 56 , 930–950.
  • Hearn, S. , Saulnier, G. , Strayer, J. , Glenham, M. , Koopman, R. , & Marcia, J. E. (2012). Between integrity and despair: Toward construct validation of Erikson’s eighth stage. Journal of Adult Development, 19 , 1–20.
  • Helson, R. , & Roberts, B. W. (1994). Ego development and personality change in adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66 , 911–920.
  • Hernandez, J. T. , & DiClemente, R. J. (1992). Self-control and ego identity development as predictors of unprotected sex in late adolescent males. Journal of Adolescence, 15 , 437–447.
  • Jespersen, K. , Kroger, J. , & Martinussen, M. (2013a). Identity status and ego development: A meta-analysis. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 13 , 228–241.
  • Jespersen, K. , Kroger, J. , & Martinussen, M. (2013b). Identity status and moral reasoning: A meta-analysis. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 13 , 266–280.
  • Jessor, R. , Turbin, M. S. , Costa, F. M. , Dong, Q. , Zhang, H. , & Wang, C. (2003). Adolescent problem behavior in China and the United States: A cross-national study of psychosocial protective factors. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 13 , 329–360.
  • Jones, R. M. , & Hartmann, B. R. (1988). Ego identity: Developmental differences and experimental substance use among adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 11 , 347–360.
  • Josselson, R. (1996). Revising herself: The story of women’s identity from college to midlife . New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Kohlberg, L. (1976). Moral stages and moralization: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In T. Lickona (Ed.), Moral development and moral behavior (pp. 31–53). New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
  • Kroger, J. (2003). What transits in an identity status transition? Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 3 , 197–220.
  • Kroger, J. , Martinussen, M. , & Marcia, J. E. (2010). Identity status change during adolescence and young adulthood: A meta-analysis. Journal of Adolescence, 33 , 683–698.
  • Kunnen, E. S. (2006). Are conflicts the motor of identity change? Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 6 , 169–186.
  • Kunnen, E. S. (2010). Characteristics and prediction of identity conflicts concerning career goals among first-year university students. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 10 , 222–231.
  • Laghi, F. , Baiocco, R. , Longiro, A. , & Baumgartner, E. (2013). Exploring the relationship between identity status development and alcohol consumption among Italian adolescents. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 147 , 277–292.
  • Lile, J. (2013). Toward a structural understanding of identity formation: A review and extension of theory. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 13 , 318–347.
  • Lile, J. (2015). The relationship of identity boundaries to identity statuses and styles: An empirical examination of the structural model of identity formation. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 14 , 113–125.
  • Lilgendahl, J. P. (2015). The dynamic role of identity processes in personality development: Theories, patterns, and new directions. In K. C. McLean & M. Syed (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of identity development (pp. 490–524). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Lillevoll, K. R. , Kroger, J. , & Martinussen, M. (2013a). Identity status and anxiety: A meta-analysis. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 13 , 214–227.
  • Lillevoll, K. R. , Kroger, J. , & Martinussen, M. (2013b). Identity status and locus of control: A meta-analysis. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 13 , 253–265.
  • Loevinger, J. (1976). Ego development: Conceptions and theories . San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
  • Luyckx, K. , Goossens, L. , Soenens, B. , & Beyers, W. (2006). Unpacking commitment and exploration: Preliminary validation of an integrative model of late adolescent identity formation. Journal of Adolescence, 29 , 361–378.
  • Luyckx, K. , Goossens, L. , Soenens, B. , Beyers, W. , Vansteenkiste, M. (2005). Identity statuses based on 4 rather than 2 identity dimensions: Extending and refining Marcia’s paradigm. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 34 , 605–618.
  • Luyckx, K. , Schwartz, S. J. , Berzonsky, M. D. , Soenens, B. , Vansteenkiste, M. , Smits, I. , et al. (2008). Capturing ruminative exploration: Extending the four-dimensional model of identity formation in late adolescence. Journal of Research in Personality, 42 , 55–82.
  • Marcia, J. E. (1966). Development and validation of ego identity status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3 , 551–558.
  • Marcia, J. E. (1980). Identity in adolescence. In J. Adelson (Ed.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (pp. 159–187). New York: Wiley.
  • Marcia, J. E. , Waterman, A. S. , Matteson, D. R. , Archer, S. L. , & Orlofsky, J. L. (1993). Ego identity: A handbook for psychosocial research . New York: Springer.
  • Martinussen, M. , & Kroger, J. (2013). Meta-analytic studies of identity status and personality: Introduction and overview. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 13 , 189–200.
  • McAdams, D. P. (2008). Personal narratives and the life story. In O. John , R. Robins , & L. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3d ed., pp. 242–262). New York: Guilford Press.
  • McLean, K. C. , & Pasupathi, M. (2012). Processes of identity development: Where I am and how I got there. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 12 , 8–28.
  • Meeus, W. , van de Schoot, R. , Keijsers, L. , Schwartz, S. J. , & Branje, S. (2010). On the progression and stability of adolescent identity formation: A fixed-wave longitudinal study in early-to-middle and middle-to-late adolescence. Child Development, 81 , 1565–1581.
  • Pulkkinen, L. , Lyyra, A. L. , Fadjukoff, P. , & Kokko, K. (2012). Parental identity in relation to social and psychological functioning in early middle age. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Devlopment. Edmonton, Canada.
  • Robinson, O. C. , & Stell, A. (2015). Later-life crisis: Towards a holistic model. Journal of Adult Development, 22 , 38–49.
  • Ryeng, M. , Kroger, J. , & Martinussen, M. (2013a). Identity status and self-esteem: A meta-analysis . Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 13 , 201–213.
  • Ryeng, M. , Kroger, J. , & Martinussen, M. (2013b). Identity status and authoritarianism: A meta-analysis . Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 13 , 242–252.
  • Schwartz, S. J. , Beyers, W. , Luyckx, K. , Soenens, B. , Zamboanga, B. L. , Forthun, L. F. , et al. (2011). Examining the light and dark sides of emerging adults’ identity: A study of identity status differences in positive and negative psychosocial functioning. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40 , 839–859.
  • Schwartz, S. J. , Pantin, H. , Prado, G. , Sullivan, S. , & Szapocznik, J. (2005). Family functioning, identity, and problem behavior in Hispanic immigrant early adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 25 , 392–420.
  • Shanahan, M. J. , & Pychyl, T. (2006). An ego identity perspective on volitional action: Identity status, agency, and procrastination. Personality and Individual Differences, 43 , 901–911.
  • Sneed, J. R. , Whitbourne, S. K. , Schwartz, S. , & Huang, S. (2011). The relationship between identity, intimacy, and midlife well-being: Findings from the Rochester Adult Longitudinal Study. Psychology and Aging, 27 , 318–323.
  • Stephen, J. , Fraser, E. , & Marcia, J. E. (1992). Moratorium–achievement (Mama) cycles in lifespan identity development: Value orientations and reasoning system correlates. Journal of Adolescence, 15 , 283–300.
  • Whitbourne, S. K. (2002). Identity processes in adulthood: Theoretical and methodological challenges. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 2 , 29–45.

Related Articles

  • Psychological Considerations of Adolescents in Sport and Performance
  • Self and Identity
  • Dynamic Integration Theory

Printed from Oxford Research Encyclopedias, Psychology. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a single article for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 28 April 2024

  • Cookie Policy
  • Privacy Policy
  • Legal Notice
  • Accessibility
  • [66.249.64.20|185.80.151.9]
  • 185.80.151.9

Character limit 500 /500

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • J Can Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
  • v.25(2); Spring 2016

Language: English | French

Links of Adolescents Identity Development and Relationship with Peers: A Systematic Literature Review

Tija ragelienė.

1 Mykolas Romeris University, Institute of Psychology, Vilnius, Lithuania

According to Erik Erikson, the main task of adolescents is to solve the crisis of identity versus role confusion. Research has shown that a stable and strong sense of identity is associated with better mental health of adolescents. Good relationships with peers are also linked with better emotional and psychological well-being of adolescents. However, there is a lack of reviews of studies in the scientific literature examining the relationship between the adolescents’ identity development and relationships with peers. The aims of this article were to analyze links between adolescent identity development and relationships with peers identified from a literature review, summarize the results, and discuss the theoretical factors that may predict these relationships.

A systematic literature review.

Analysis of findings from the systematic literature review revealed that a good relationship with peers is positively related to adolescent identity development, but empirical research in this area is extremely limited.

Conclusions:

The links between adolescents’ identity development and their relationship with peers are not completely clear. The possible intermediate factors that could determine the relationship between adolescent identity development and their relationships with peers are discussed. Further empirical researches is needed in this area.

Résumé

Selon Erik Erikson, la principale tâche des adolescents consiste à résoudre la crise d’identité versus la confusion des rôles. La recherche a démontré qu’un sentiment d’identité fort et stable est associé à une meilleure santé mentale des adolescents. De bonnes relations avec les pairs sont également liées à un plus grand bien-être émotionnel et psychologique des adolescents. Toutefois, il y a un manque de revues d’études dans la littérature scientifique qui examine la relation entre le développement de l’identité des adolescents et les relations avec les pairs. Cet article visait à analyser les liens entre le développement de l’identité des adolescents et les relations avec les pairs identifiés dans une revue de la littérature, à résumer les résultats, et à discuter des facteurs théoriques qui peuvent prédire ces relations.

Méthode:

Une revue systématique de la littérature.

Résultats:

L’analyse des résultats de la revue systématique de la littérature a révélé qu’une bonne relation avec les pairs est positivement reliée au développement de l’identité de l’adolescent, mais que la recherche empirique dans ce domaine est extrêmement limitée.

Les liens entre le développement de l’identité des adolescents et leurs relations avec les pairs ne sont pas complètement définis. Les facteurs intermédiaires possibles qui pourraient déterminer la relation entre le développement de l’identité des adolescents et leurs relations avec les pairs sont présentés. Il faut plus de recherches empiriques dans ce domaine.

Introduction

According to Erikson (1968) , the main and most important developmental tasks for adolescents are to solve the identity versus role confusion crisis, construct their own unique sense of identity, and find the social environment where they can belong to and create meaningful relationships with other people ( Chen, Lay, Wu, & Yao, 2007 ). Various theories exist that try to define the concept of identity. For example, Waterman (1993; as cited in Guardia, 2009 ) believed that identity reflects a person’s best potential which is self-realizing. Identity is expressed through self-realizing activities which person feels as self-defining or fitting, engaging, energizing, purposeful, and helpful in fulfilling his/her goals of life. According to Berzonsky’s social-cognitive theory of identity styles (1988; as cited in Guardia, 2009 ), identity is the cognitive model of how persons process and examines identity relevant information. Others propose that identity in general is nothing other than the answer to the question: “who am I?” ( Motyl, 2010 ). Identity theory developed by Stryker (1968; as cited in Adamsons & Pasley, 2013 ) focuses on the individual’s integration of societal expectations regarding the meaning of occupying particular social positions or statuses (for example, parent, spouse, employee) and the behavioural expectations associated with such statuses. The most general and broad concept of identity is proposed by Erikson (1968) . He described identity as a fundamental organizing principal which develops constantly throughout the lifespan. Identity provides a sense of continuity within the self and in interaction with others (“self-sameness”), as well as a frame to differentiate between self and others (“uniqueness”) which allows the individual to function autonomously from others ( Erikson, 1968 ). Moreover, Erikson’s concept of identity was particularly focused on adolescents’ development. For these reasons identity definition based on Erikson’s theory of psychosocial development was used in this study.

Both, the high-quality and satisfying relationships with other people ( Walsh, Harel-Fisch, & Fogel-Grinvalds, 2010 ) and the formed sense of identity ( Dumas, Lawford, Tieu, & Pratt, 2009 ) are associated with better adolescent’ mental health and psychological well-being. A positively solved identity vs. role confusion crisis enables a person to integrate self-perception images into a stable personal identity, as well as strengthen performance of different roles and assumed commitments ( Erikson, 1968 ). Achieved identity is associated with a lower expression of psychosomatic and neurotic symptoms ( Chen et al., 2007 ), and reduced anxiety ( Crocetti, Klimstra, Keijsers, Hale, & Meeus, 2009 ), depression and suicidal tendencies ( Ramgoon, Bachoo, Patel, & Wig, 2006 ). Achieved identity is also related to psychological well-being ( Sandhu, Singh, Tung & Kundra, 2012 ), emotional adjustment ( Dumas et al., 2009 ), and greater emotional stability ( Crocetti, Rubini, Luyckx, & Meeus, 2008 ).

Similarly, relationships with peers are also associated with better mental health of adolescents and his/her successful adaptation to the environment ( La Greca & Harrison, 2005 ; Yeung & Leadbeater, 2010 ). Although relationships with parents are still relevant for adolescents ( Eckstein, Rasmussen, & Wittschen, 1999 ), relationships with peers become the centre of adolescents’ life ( Tarrant, 2002 ). Research has shown that belonging to the “normal” - or - popular peer group is positively associated with better adolescent adaptation to the environment ( Heaven, Ciarrochi, & Cechaviciute, 2005 ). Research has shown that peer group acceptance and good relationships work as protecting factors against social anxiety and depression ( La Greca, & Harrison, 2005 ). It is also related with less internal and external problems regardless of adolescent’ sex, age and family structure characteristics ( Newman, Lohman, & Newman, 2007 ). If adolescents are unable to resist peer pressure and negative influences, they may be more prone to delinquent behaviour or use of psychoactive substances. This may be so as, adolescents may seek to conform to behavioural norms of the peer group with, which they identify themselves with ( Stewart-Knox et al., 2005 ). Identification with a delinquent peer group is a predictor of physical and relationship aggression ( Pokhrel, Sussman, Black, & Sun, 2010 ). Aggression experienced in adolescents’ romantic relationships, peer pressure, and peer rejection are associated with increased symptoms of depression, alcohol use, psychosocial functioning problems ( Schad, Szwed, Antonishak, Hare, & Allen 2008 ), internal distress and social anxiety ( La Greca & Harrison, 2005 ).

In adolescence, a self-comparison with peers becomes more important when adolescents try to associate their identities with their peers ( Hill, Bromell, Tyson, & Flint, 2007 ). Members of the peer group can model their behaviour according to group norms’, therefore peer groups can have significant impact on adolescents’ development ( Steinberg & Morris, 2001 ). A peer group not only provides emotional support for adolescents, but also provides a social status necessary for his/her identity development ( Nawaz, 2011 ). However, it is not clear what psychosocial factors may link adolescent identity development and relationship with peers.

Research has shown that adolescents’ identity development is related to their relationship with peers. According to Rassart and colleagues (Rassart, Luyckx, Apers, Goossens, & Moons, 2012 ), good and supporting relationship with peers is positively related to adolescent identity development because it can help prevent stagnation in the process of identity exploration. Attachment to peers and belonging to peer groups, as well as the support one get from romantic relationships and good relationships with friends, are also positively related to adolescent identity development ( Nawaz, 2011 ; Pugh & Hart, 1999 ; Klimstra et al., 2013 ; Meeus, Oosterwegel, & Vollebergh 2002 ). However, appears that only a few studies examining the links between adolescent identity development and relationships with peers has been published in the peer-reviewed literature. Studies that have been published appear to show that high-quality relationships with peers are positively related to adolescent identity development. However, the dimensions of relationships with peers discussed in these studies substantially vary, ranging from intimate romantic relationships to membership within wider social peer groups. In addition, these studies are mainly based on correlation analysis, which cannot determine causal relationships between the variables, and also do not address what other factors could predict the links of adolescent identity development and relationships with peers. In order to summarize the research about adolescent identity development in the context of relationship with peers, a systematic literature review was conducted. The main tasks of this literature review were to: summarize results of adolescents’ identity development and relationships with peers; and, discuss the theoretical factors that could predict the links of adolescent identity development and relationships with peers.

Methodology

Inclusion criteria for the systematic literature review.

Only empirical studies which examined:

  • adolescent’ identity development according to Erickson’s theory of psychosocial development; and,
  • various aspects of adolescents’ relationships with peers were included in the systematic literature review. In addition, studies were only included if the participants were non-clinical samples.

Excluded were studies that only presented theoretical content and studies that only examined gender, racial or ethnic identity because gender, racial and ethnic identity are more reflective of different types of identity, rather than identity itself. Other inclusion criteria were studies’ availability, quality, language and year of publication: only freely available, peer-reviewed, full-text articles in English, which were published between 1985 and 2014, were included. The following databases were searched: Medline, Social Psychology Network, Google Scholar and EBSCO. The following combinations of key words were used: identity, development, adolescent *, relationship *, peer *, friend *. In total 53 articles were found: 16 – in Medline, 0 –in Social Psychology Network, 16 – in Google Scholar and 26 – in EBSCO. All articles were screened according to titles, abstracts and used methodologies. Articles, which did not match inclusion criteria, were not included into systematic review.

Assessment of study quality and data extraction

The quality of the studies was assessed according to the following criteria to determine which of the 53 studies to retain for analysis:

  • Adolescent’s identity development was analyzed in accordance with Erikson psychosocial development theory;
  • Information about the participants of the study was provided;
  • Participants were non-clinical samples;
  • Research concepts were clearly defined;
  • Reliability coefficients of used instruments were given;
  • Organization and implementation of the research were described;
  • Obtained results and findings of the study were clearly described.

In total seven of the 53 studies identified in the systematic literature review met the quality criteria: four – from EBSCO database and three – from Google’s Scholar search engine. The data about participants’ age and reliability of used instruments were extracted. The data from the analysis of the links between adolescent’ relationships with peers and identity development were also extracted. Different methods of assessment of identity development and relationships with peers were used across the included studies, therefore meta-analytic calculations were not performed.

In total seven articles identified through the systematic literature review met the inclusion and quality criteria. The main characteristics of these studies are presented in Table 1 . There were different methodologies used in assessing adolescent identity development and relationships with peers in the included studies. Adolescents’ relationships with peers were most frequently analyzed by measuring attachment to peers (4 of 7, #2, #4, #6, #7). Other relationships considered in one study each included: orientation to peer group (#1), having a romantic relationship (#3), conformist behaviour (#5), and adjustment to peers (#5). Reliability coefficients of the measures used in the various studies met a proposed threshold indicating suitability for use in psychological research for group comparisons (i.e., Cronbach α> 0.5) ( Streiner & Norman, 2008 ).

Characteristics of the population and measures used in the studies identified in the systematic literature review

Findings from the included studies are summarized in Table 2 . In the majority of studies (6 of 7, #1–4, #6–7) statistically significant links between adolescent identity development and relationships with peers were found. Achieved sense of identity is positively associated with attachment to peers (#2, #4, #6, #7), adolescents’ orientation to the formal peer groups’ achievements (#1), stability and robustness in romantic relationships (#3), better relationships with peers, and reduced sense of loneliness (#7). In contrast, mixed findings were found in one study. More specifically, Adams et al. (1985) (#5) found achievement of identity is associated with better interpersonal characteristics (attentiveness to social information, capability of addressing numerous ideas and thoughts, being less controlling and expressing less negative emotions in the context of interpersonal relationships) while conformist behaviour with peers is not predicted by identity statuses.

Summary of links between adolescent identity development and relationships with peers from studies identified in the systematic literature review

The importance of relationships with peers increases markedly in adolescence ( Tarrant, 2002 ) and is related with adolescent identity development ( Pugh & Hart, 1999 ). However, based on this systematic review there is an overall lack of studies analyzing links between adolescents’ relationships with peers and their identity development using Erikson’s theory of psychosocial development. In addition, the different contexts, methodologies, and measures used in the included studies leave uncertainties in a number of areas. First, the links of identity development and relationship with peers were analyzed in samples of participants of different age and contexts. The ages ranged from 14 to 25 years. Analyzed aspects of relationship with peers also varied including relationships with friends, classmates or other peers in the school, and relationship with romantic partners. Given these variations, there are still questions as to “how identity development is related with adolescents’ age and specificity of relationship type with peers”. Second, the included studies were primarily based on correlation analysis and thus not able to determine causal relationships between variables.

Results of the systematic literature review found that adolescent identity development is positively associated with an attachment to peers ( Nawaz, 2011 ; Rassart et al., 2012 ; Meeus et al., 2002 ; Doumen et al., 2012 ), a better quality of relationship with peers and a lower tendency to experience negative feelings such as loneliness ( Doumen et al., 2012 ). The reached identity is also positively related to being less nervous or competitive in interpersonal relations ( Adams et al., 1985 ) and being more satisfied in romantic relationships ( Klimstra et al., 2013 ). In addition, reached identify is positively related to the ability to reflect on one’s interpersonal relationships and to be less controlling ( Adams et al., 1985 ).

Desire to belong to a peer group and active involvement and participation in various peer groups activities are also linked with a reached identity ( Pugh & Hart, 1999 ). This could be explained by the fact that peer groups become the place where adolescents can compare themselves with each other and achieve a personal sense of identity ( Nawaz, 2011 ) which may be constructed in the context of social relationships. Participation in new activities and new experiences in the peer group allows adolescents to explore different values and norms. Discussions with peers may also help them to understand themselves better. In this way, adolescents could construct their identity by adopting certain values and norms of their peer group and rejecting others, which they have found as unacceptable with their world-view. Social interactions with peers with similar or different values also could help adolescents to clarify their world-view and values. This could be so as, communication with peers could give adolescents a supportive social environment where they can freely discuss what they are and who they would like to be. The peer group where adolescents belong can also provide values and norms which adolescents can identify with ( Pugh & Hart, 1999 ). By communicating with peers, adolescents can not only discuss their identity issues with significant others, but can get their approval and in turn achieve identity more easily ( Rassart et al., 2012 ). The fact that a high-quality relationship with peers is associated with the achieved identity could also be related to the fact that a good relationship between peers characterized by mutual cooperation, negotiation, and mutual understanding, helps adolescents to explore their identity with minimal risk to their self-image, which, in turn, may stimulate adolescents to commit to their goals and beliefs. In this way, good relationship with peers can enhance adolescent’s self-esteem and help to define themselves and work as a protecting factor against the anxiety related with identity development ( Rassart et al., 2012 ). Results of this study revealed that achieved identity is related to better relationships with peers. Being a part of a peer group, good, positive communication with peers may provide appropriate social context for adolescent’s personal identity development. Although the results of the systematic literature review do not disclose causal relationship between adolescent identity development and relationships with peers.

Despite identifying links between adolescent identity development and relationship with peers from the literature review, some theoretical ambiguities remain. In many of the analyzed studies (study #1–4, #6) it is not clear what other factors could be related or predict quality of relationships with peers and how these other factors might link with adolescent identity development. Adams & Ryan (1985) (study #5) and, Doumen and colleagues (2012) (study #7) touch on personality traits that may be related to identity development, but these links are mentioned only secondarily (study #5) or only as very specific characteristics, for example, anxiety or tendency to experience more loneliness (study #7). However, it still remains unclear what other factors could be related with adolescent identity development and quality of relationships with peers. Some additional factors, that are often examined in the context of relationships and are associated with quality of relationships, could be adolescents’ ability to be empathic and differentiation of self.

Empathy is an essential communication skill that helps to understand the views of others and to predict their emotional reactions. Empathy is most commonly defined as the emotional trait which helps to facilitate the other person’s emotional experience. It is also the cognitive skill that facilitates other person’s emotion understanding (Hogan, 1969 and Mehrabian & Epstein, 1972, as cited in Jolliffe & Farrington, 2007 ). Empathy can also be defined as a relatively neutral or pleasant feeling for others that awakens altruistic motivation to reduce distress or meet the needs of other persons ( Ocejo, López-Pérez, Ambrona, & Fernández, 2009 ). High levels of adolescent empathy are associated with a reduced experience of bullying and, a desire to protect victims of bullying ( Gini, Albiero, Benelli, & Altoe, 2007 ). It is also related to adolescents’ pro-social behaviour and, positive evaluations of the quality of their relationship with peers ( Barr & Higgins-Dalessandro, 2007 ). Higher self-control, desire to cooperate, decrease conflict with peers (Mount, 2010) and helping others in emergency situations ( Masten, Eisenberger, Pfeifer, & Dapretto, 2010 ) are also linked with empathy. Research shown that empathy is also associated with a successful adaptation in social relationships and more developed social competence ( Glick & Rose, 2011 ), as well as better communication skills in adolescence and adulthood ( Allemand, Steiger, & Fend, 2014 ). In considering links between empathy and identity development Smits, Doumen, Luyckx, Duriez, & Goossens (2011) have found that empathy can be predicted by information-oriented and diffuse-avoidant identity styles, and empathy can predict adolescent interpersonal behaviour in social relationships. It means that empathy mediates relationship between adolescent information-oriented and diffuse-avoidant identity styles and behaviour in interpersonal relationships. Therefore, it could be that empathy may also be one of the intermediate factors that links adolescents’ identity development and relationships with peers and be in alignment with the finding from the systematic literature review that good relationships with peers are important for adolescent identity development ( Doumen et al., 2012 ; Klimstra et al., 2013 ; Meeus et al., 2002 ; Nawaz, 2011 ; Rassart et al., 2012 ).

Differentiation of self is a personality factor which is associated with better interpersonal relationships, adaptive functioning in the environment ( Jenkins, Buboltz, Schwartz, & Johnson, 2005 ), lower anxiety levels ( Skowron & Dendy, 2004 ) and greater psychological well-being ( Skowron, Stanley & Shapiro, 2009 ). The concept of differentiation of self becomes important in adolescence when the adolescent seeks to create their identity in the context of relationships with peers. Many adolescents tend to copy the behaviour of their friends or other important people in order to adapt to peers and/or to belong to a peer group. Through communication with peers adolescents have an opportunity to check their beliefs about themselves and the world and compare it with their peers. In communication processes adolescents also gets an opportunity to discuss, oppose and change their thinking about the world and themselves which is essential to identity development. Peer groups also provides values and norms which adolescents can identify with ( Pugh & Hart, 1999 ). Susceptibility to peer pressure and conformism become problematic when adolescents encourage each other to participate in antisocial, or deviant activities such as smoking, substance use, early sexual behavior or bullying ( Chan & Chan, 2008 ). The concept of differentiation of self, developed by Bowen (1978) , involves a person’s ability to distinguish him or herself as a separate, autonomous individual from other persons. Individuals with a high level of differentiation of self are able to perceive and distinguish their thoughts, opinions and worldview from other people and understand the other person’s position simultaneously. Such a person is also able to share his/her experiences, feelings, opinions and respected differences with other persons and accept them at the same time. A person with a high differentiation of self is also aware of his/her autonomy and personality boundaries and is able to take responsibility for his/her behaviour ( Bowen, 1978 ). High differentiation of self is related to a better ability to create and maintain interpersonal relationships ( Lal & Bartle-Haring, 2011 ; Holman & Busby, 2011 ). In adolescence, differentiation of self is important in order to establish and maintain good relationships with peers, realize one’s unique place within relationships, and to be able to resist negative pressure of peers. Thus, differentiation of self could be a second factor which could be related to successful adolescent’ relationships with peers and how he/she will be able to resist the negative values, group norms and behaviours of peers that he/she does not agree. Therefore adolescents’ differentiation of self could also be one of the intermediate factors that could link adolescent’ identity formation and relationships with peers and could predict direction and strength of these links. In order to examine these interrelationships, further empirical research in this area is needed.

Conclusions

  • Adolescents’ identity development is positively related with their relationships with peers. Belonging to a peer group and good relationships with peers based on mutual respect and acceptance are positively related with adolescent identity development.
  • Personality related factors that could be linked with adolescents’ identity development and their relationship with peers are not clear. Potential factors of interest, which are associated with good social relationships, are adolescent’s ability to be empathic and his/her differentiation of self. Further empirical research investigating links between adolescent identity development, empathy, differentiation of self, and relationships with peers is needed.

Acknowledgements/Conflicts of Interest

The author has no conflicts to disclose.

  • Adams GR, Ryan JH, Hoffman J, Dobson WR, Nielsen E. Ego identity status, conformity behavior, and personality in late adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1985; 47 :1091–1104. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Adamsons K, Pasley K. Refining identity theory to better account for relationships and context: Applications to fathering. Journal of Family Theory & Review. 2013; 5 :159–175. doi: 10.1111/jftr.12014. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Allemand M, Steiger AE, Fend HA. Empathy development in adolescence predicts social competencies in adulthood. Journal of Personality. 2014:1–13. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barr J, Higgins-Dalessandro A. Adolescent empathy and prosocial behavior in the multidimensional context of school culture. The Journal of Genetic Psychology. 2007; 168 :231–250. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bowen M. Family therapy in clinical practice. New York, NY: Library of Congress Catalog, Copyright by J. Aronson; 1978. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chan K, Chan S. Emotional Autonomy versus susceptibility to peer pressure a case study of hong kong adolescent students. Research in Education. 2008; 79 :39–55. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chen K, Lay K, Wu Y, Yao G. Adolescent self-identity and mental health: The function of identity importance, identity firmness, and identity discrepancy. Chinese Journal of Psychology. 2007; 49 :53–72. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crocetti E, Klimstra T, Keijsers L, Hale W, Meeus W. Anxiety trajectories and identity development in adolescence: A five-wave longitudinal study. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2009; 38 :839–849. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crocetti E, Rubini M, Luyckx K, Meeus W. Identity formation in early and middle adolescents from various ethnic groups: From three dimensions to five statuses. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2008; 37 :983–996. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Doumen S, Smits I, Luyckx K, Duriez B, Vanhalst J, Verschueren K, Goossens L. Identity and perceived peer relationship quality in emerging adulthood: The mediating role of attachment-related emotions. Journal of Adolescence. 2012; 35 :1417–1425. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dumas TM, Lawford H, Tieu T, Pratt MW. Positive parenting and its relation to identity status in young adulthood: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology. 2009; 45 :1531–1544. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eckstein DG, Rasmussen PR, Wittschen L. Understanding and dealing with adolescent. The Journal of Individual Psychology. 1999; 55 :1–21. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Erikson EH. Identity, youth and crisis. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company, Inc; 1968. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gini G, Albiero P, Benelli B, Altoe G. Does empathy predict adolescents’ bullying and defending behavior? Aggressive Behavior. 2007; 33 :467–476. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Glick G, Rose AJ. Prospective associations between friendship adjustment and social strategies: Friendship as a context for building social skills. Developmental Psychology. 2011; 47 :1117–1132. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Guardia JG. Developing who I am: A self-determination theory approach to the establishment of healthy identities. Educational Psychologist. 2009; 44 :90–104. doi: 10.1080/00461520902832350. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Heaven PC, Ciarrochi J, Cechaviciute I. Adolescent peer crowd self-identification, attributional style and perceptions of parenting. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology. 2005; 15 :313–318. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hill N, Bromell L, Tyson D, Flint R. Developmental commentary: Ecological perspectives on parental influences during adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2007; 36 :367–377. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Holman TB, Busby DM. Family-of-origin, differentiation of self and partner, and adult romantic relationship quality. Journal of Couple & Relationship Therapy. 2011; 10 :3–19. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jenkins SM, Buboltz WC, Schwartz JP, Johnson P. Differentiation of self and psychosocial development. Contemporary Family Therapy. 2005; 27 :249–259. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jolliffe D, Farrington DP. Examining the relationship between low empathy and self-reported offending. Legal and Criminological Psychology. 2007; 12 :265–286. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Klimstra T, Luyckx K, Branje S, Teppers E, Goossens L, Meeus W. Personality traits, interpersonal identity, and relationship stability: Longitudinal linkages in late adolescence and young adulthood. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2013; 42 :1661–1673. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • La Greca AM, Harrison HM. Adolescent peer relations, friendships, and romantic relationships: Do they predict social anxiety and depression? Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2005; 34 :49–61. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lal A, Bartle-Haring S. Relationship among differentiation of self, relationship satisfaction, partner support, and depression in patients with chronic lung disease and their partners. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy. 2011; 37 :121–271. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Masten CL, Eisenberger NI, Pfeifer JH, Dapretto M. Witnessing peer rejection during adolescence: Neural correlates of empathy for experiences of social exclusion. Social Neuroscience. 2010; 5 :496–507. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Meeus W, Oosterwegel A, Vollebergh A. Parental and peer attachment and identity development in adolescence. Journal of Adolescence. 2002; 25 :93–106. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Motyl AJ. The social construction of social construction: Implications for theories of nationalism and identity formation. Nationalities Papers. 2010; 38 :59–71. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nawaz S. The relationship of parental and peer attachment bonds with the identity development during adolescence. Journal of Social Sciences. 2011; 5 :104–119. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Newman BM, Lohman BJ, Newman PR. Peer group membership and a sense of belonging: Their relationship to adolescent behavior problems. Adolescence. 2007; 42 :1–24. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Oceja L, López-Pérez B, Ambrona T, Fernández I. Measuring general dispositions to feeling empathy and distress. Psicothema. 2009; 21 :171–176. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pokhrel P, Sussman S, Black D, Sun P. Peer group self identification as a predictor of relational and physical aggression among high school students. Journal of School Health. 2010; 80 :249–258. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pugh M, Hart D. Identity development and peer group participation. New Direction for Child and Adolescent Development. 1999; 84 :55–72. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ramgoon S, Bachoo Sh, Patel C, Paruk Z. Could a healthy ego identity save as a protective factor against suicidal tendencies? A pilot study. Journal of Child and Adolescent Mental Health. 2006; 18 :49–54. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rassart J, Luyckx K, Apers S, Goossens E, Moons P. Identity dynamics and peer relationship quality in adolescents with a chronic disease: The sample case of congenital heart disease. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 2012; 33 :625–632. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sandhu D, Singh B, Tung S, Kundra N. Adolescent identity formation, psychological well-being, and parental attitudes. Pakistan Journal Of Psychological Research. 2012; 27 :89–105. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schad MM, Szwedo DE, Antonishak J, Hare A, Allen JP. The broader context of relational aggression in adolescent romantic relationships: Predictions from peer pressure and links to psychosocial functioning. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2008; 37 (3):346–358. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Skowron EA, Dendy AK. Differentiation of self and attachment in adulthood: Relational correlates of effortful control. Contemporary Family Therapy. 2004; 26 :337–357. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Skowron EA, Stanley KL, Shapiro MD. A longitudinal perspective on differentiation of self, interpersonal and psychological well-being in young adulthood. Contemporary Family Therapy. 2009; 31 :3–18. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Smits I, Doumen S, Luyckx K, Duriez B, Goossens L. Identity styles and interpersonal behavior in emerging adulthood: The intervening role of empathy. Social Development. 2011; 20 :664–686. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Steinberg L, Morris AS. Adolescent development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2001; 52 :83–110. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stewart-Knox BJ, Sittlington J, Rugkåsa J, Harrisson Sh, Treacy M, Abaunza PS. Smoking and peer groups: Results from a longitudinal qualitative study of young people in Northern Ireland. British Journal of Social Psychology. 2005; 44 :397–414. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Streiner LD, Norman GR. Health measurement scales: A practical guide to their development and use. 4th ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2008. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tarrant M. Adolescent peer groups and social identity. Social Development. 2002; 11 :110–124. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Walsh SD, Harel-Fisch Y, Fogel-Grinvald H. Parents, teachers and peer relations as predictors of risk behaviors and mental well-being among immigrant and israeli born adolescents. Social Science & Medicine. 2010; 70 :976–984. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yeung R, Leadbeater B. Adults make a difference: The protective effects of parent and teacher emotional support on emotional and behavioral problems of peer-victimized adolescents. Journal of Community Psychology. 2010; 38 :80–98. [ Google Scholar ]

The Crisis of Identity

  • First Online: 02 September 2020

Cite this chapter

identity crisis research paper

  • Yvan Yenda Ilunga 3  

Part of the book series: Critical Political Theory and Radical Practice ((CPTRP))

147 Accesses

This chapter offers the view that long-term recovery and humanitarian solutions will be incomplete and unsuccessful if the identity issue among vulnerable populations, mainly displaced populations, is not central to the debate and practice of humanitarian action. In this chapter, Ilunga argues that identity issues should be addressed by tackling the problems of denationalization, de-patriotization, and hybrid identity. In fact, these three manifestations of the identity crisis continue to weaken the already challenged notion of national identity in Africa, which by its nature is so often undermined by the predominance of ethnicity. To address this crisis, the author suggests the use of Pan-Africanism as an inclusive ideology and identity, as supported by outstanding Pan-Africanist figures such as Bantu Stephen Biko and Kwame Nkrumah. Embracing Pan-Africanism has the merit of addressing the national and regional security crisis, while at the same time promoting an inclusive continental identity that would counteract the identity crisis engendered by humanitarian chaos.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
  • Durable hardcover edition

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Political Science, James Madison University, Harrisonburg, VA, USA

Yvan Yenda Ilunga

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yvan Yenda Ilunga .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Ilunga, Y.Y. (2020). The Crisis of Identity. In: Humanitarianism and Security. Critical Political Theory and Radical Practice. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-51689-5_3

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-51689-5_3

Published : 02 September 2020

Publisher Name : Palgrave Macmillan, Cham

Print ISBN : 978-3-030-51688-8

Online ISBN : 978-3-030-51689-5

eBook Packages : Political Science and International Studies Political Science and International Studies (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

Realizing R&B's Identity Crisis: An Analytical Research Paper

Profile image of Jasmine A . Henry

Although scholars and music industry professionals have expressed their criticisms on the current state of R&B, as far as we know, R&B’s identity crisis has not been critically examined in an academic setting. However, an analysis of the social and musical factors contributing to R&B’s ongoing identity crisis reveals that many popular recording artists stretch the artistic and musical boundaries of contemporary R&B by continuously gravitating towards and moving away from the genre. More specifically, this paper will use qualitative and historical research methods to evaluate how R&B is affected by genre interplay, racial issues, and the rise of alternative R&B—in an attempt to gain a further understanding of the genre’s current identity crisis and highlight areas for future research.

Related Papers

MEIKA J COLE

This paper engages in a careful examination of how African-American identity has been impacted by popular culture, specifically the commercialization of black music. Over the last 30 years, the soul of black music has been replaced with a more homogenized sound that embodies the characteristics of popular music, as defined by a European ideology. As the genre gained crossover acceptance in the 1940’s, texts that once signified black cultural identity and family unity have now been replaced with those of extreme materialism, violence and promiscuity. The more commercialized the genre becomes, the message of love, freedom and self-expression for the black community gets more diluted leaving me to question whether or not the soul of black music is dead and if it can ever be revived.

identity crisis research paper

The SAGE Handbook of Popular Music

Jon Stratton

Feminist Media Studies

Timothy N Laurie

This article examines the cultural politics of “crossover” at Motown Records, focusing on the relationship between genre, gender, and career longevity. Beginning with the Supremes’ covers albums in the mid-1960s, the article links notions of musical originality to commercial logics of publishing, gendered divisions of labour, and racialised channels of record distribution. It also traces the rise of the celebrity songwriter-producer in soul, including artists like Isaac Hayes, Norman Whitfield, and Stevie Wonder, who fit a new mould of artistic authenticity that clashed with the carefully manicured performances of 1960s “girl pop.” The professional mobility afforded to men in both rock and r&b should prompt media scholars to consider the temporal dimensions of artist trajectories in the music industry, and taking the constraints on girl group singers seriously allows for reflection on (gendered) music industry knowledge about which audiences matter and for how long.

Songs of Earth. Aesthetic & Social Codes in Music

Anna L C Wood

This volume brings together for the first time book chapters, articles and position pieces from the debates on music and identity, which seek to answer classic questions such as: how has music shaped the ways in which we understand our identities and those of others? In what ways has scholarly writing about music dealt with identity politics since the Second World War? Both classic and more recent contributions are included, as well as material on related issues such as music's role as a resource in making and performing identities and music scholarship's ambivalent relationship with scholarly activism and identity politics. The essays approach the music-identity relationship from a wide range of methodological perspectives, ranging from critical historiography and archival studies, psychoanalysis, gender and sexuality studies, to ethnography and anthropology, and social and cultural theories drawn from sociology; and from continental philosophy and Marxist theories of class to a range of globalization theories. The collection draws on the work of Anglophone scholars from all over the globe, and deals with a wide range of musics and cultures, from the Americas, Australasia, Europe, the Middle East and Africa. This unique collection of key texts, which deal not just with questions of gender, sexuality and race, but also with other socially-mediated identities such as social class, disability, national identity and accounts and analyses of inter-group encounters, is an invaluable resource for music scholars and researchers and those working in any discipline that deals with identity or identity politics.

Michel Nicolau Netto

This article investigates the definitions of &quot;particular&quot; and &quot;universal&quot; found in the music industry, especialy the so-called &quot;World Music&quot; industry. It will be demonstrated that since theses definitions are not constant, they can be used for creating differences and similarities, wich produce social impact. In the case os World Music elaborated inthis study, I argue that the discourse of diversity divides musicians into &quot;flexible&quot; or &quot;fixed&quot; actors. Accordingly, these distinct categories of actors also possess varying degrees of facility when dealing with their identity and negotianting the value of that identity; consequently, musicians are faced with unequal conditions and oportunities.

Shondrea Thornton

For Black women in America, the political landscapes of feminism, race, and sexuality have always represented a sensitive balancing act. Considered “other” within the hegemonic structures of white-supremacist patriarchy, Black women have historically confronted and combated a myriad of stereotypes, using the tools afforded to them, as they faced specific challenges that placed them in subordinate position for equity and opportunity. Music, particularly blues, offered a platform unlike any other available as its creation lent itself to the understanding of recurring themes, ideas, and multiple frames of consciousness that existed in both the social and private spheres of politicized Black life. My research makes an intervention by bridging this gap and exploring contemporary manifestations of this legacy. By choosing Diana Ross, Whitney Houston, and Beyoncé, and bringing their art to the forefront of conversation, I am representing almost five decades of musical innovation and creation. With a focus on the role of popular culture in identity politics and cultural formation, I use close readings of their art as well as analysis of music charts, journalism, and reviews, to explore the influence of traditional blues culture, Black feminisms, and contemporary politics on the work produced by the three while providing a frame in which to explore implications of contemporary popular culture on identity formation and representations of racialized womanhood.

Aaron Klassen

Naomi Andre

and their agents are not liable for any legal actions that may arise involving the article&#39;s content, including but not limited to, copyright infringement. The title of this collection, Music and the Racial Imagination, is provocative—and I&#39;ll wager that this is intentional. It is a great title because it simultaneously does several things. It brings together &quot; music &quot; and &quot; race, &quot; which have not previously been given equal emphasis in musicology, and then links these two things with imagination. 1 The title creates an imagined space where music and race can begin to co-star in leading roles. As music specialists who feel connected to the world in which we live and wish that music scholarship had been more cognizant of current issues sooner, we read the book&#39;s title and think &quot; Finally! Someone is taking this on. &quot; The co-editors of this collection, Ronald Radano and Philip V. Bohlman, do not balk and seem to be aware of what they are getti...

hector qirko

This chapter argues that contrary to conventional wisdom and the views of many scholars and critics, rock and roll’s most important rhythmic elements are derived from a complex blend of African, American, and European musical traditions. There is therefore no objective support for racialized characterizations of the “beat” that often serve, however unintentionally, to perpetuate stereotypes in discussions of both the genre and American popular music more

RELATED PAPERS

Muhammad Rasyid

Mazharul Islam

Turkish Journal of Entomology

özlem sönmezoğlu

angelo giardino

Journal of Applied Physiology

Alice Babin

Gyula Kedves

Frank Koonce

SIGraDi 2021 | Designing Possibilities | Ubiquitous Conference

Jeffrey Collins

Cement and Concrete Composites

ahmed khalifa

Carlos Vàsquez Peña

2007 IEEE International Conference on Image Processing

fatma arslan

Journal of Geophysical Research

International Journal of Climatology

Walter Vanegas Vargas

Obatherbal Sarafkejepit

Hans-walter Rix

Environmental Geotechnics

Alfonso Rodríguez Dono

JOURNAL OBGIN EMAS

Bobby Indra W

Molecular Biology and Evolution

Giovanni Destro Bisol

Khazanah: Jurnal Sejarah dan Kebudayaan Islam

Khairul Amal

Lisa Pytlikzillig

International Journal of Applied Pharmaceutics

International Journal of Applied Pharmaceutics (IJAP)

International Journal of Mechanical Sciences

Buddhika Abeyrathna

Revista Integración y Cooperación Internacional

Melisa Wilson

hjjkhjhg gghgt

Neil Danielson

RELATED TOPICS

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

IMAGES

  1. (PDF) Identity Crisis (discussion paper)

    identity crisis research paper

  2. (PDF) Identity/Crisis

    identity crisis research paper

  3. Signs of an Identity Crisis & What to Do About It

    identity crisis research paper

  4. Personal Identity Essay Free Essay Example

    identity crisis research paper

  5. Erikson's Identity Crisis Essay

    identity crisis research paper

  6. (PDF) Identity Crisis? Problems and Issues in the Sociology of Education

    identity crisis research paper

VIDEO

  1. Identity crisis percussion ensemble

  2. The Identity Crisis and Truth

COMMENTS

  1. (PDF) What is an Identity Crisis?

    This identity crisis is not. merely an unfelt discontinuity in numerical identity as the ones. described in fission cases invok ed by person al identity theor ists. (Nozick, 1981; Parfit, 1984 ...

  2. The Development of Self and Identity in Adolescence

    The range of simple value inputs in much of the relevant research cited previously was limited; researchers should therefore expand the set of stimulus types used in experiments to include more complex, identity-relevant targets and ecologically valid decision-making contexts. ... Identity: Youth and crisis. New York, NY: Norton. [Google ...

  3. Dynamics of Identity Development in Adolescence: A Decade in Review

    In the current review, we discuss progress in the field of identity research between 2010 and 2020. One of the key developmental tasks in adolescence and young adulthood is to develop a coherent sense of self and identity (Erikson, 1968).Personal identity refers to one's sense of the person one genuinely is, including a subjective feeling of self-sameness and continuity over contexts and time.

  4. Identity in young adulthood: Links with mental health and risky

    Identity and psychosocial outcomes. Erikson (1950, 1968) emphasized, identity is important not only in terms of how it unfolds during the transition to adulthood, but also in terms of how it relates to important mental health outcomes.Some of the domains of functioning to which identity has been found to be related include well-being, internalizing symptoms, externalizing problems, and health ...

  5. Identity Development in Adolescence and Adulthood

    While some early research has estimated the likelihood of a midlife identity crisis to be about 10% (e.g., Brim, 1992), recent work has pointed to ongoing times of identity crisis (or revision) during the later adult years as well (Robinson & Stell, 2015).

  6. The Role of School in Adolescents' Identity Development. A Literature

    Schools can play an important role in adolescents' identity development. To date, research on the role of school in adolescents' identity development is scattered across research fields that employ different theoretical perspectives on identity. ... Research Papers in Education, 24(4), 477-497. ... Identity: youth and crisis. New York: W ...

  7. Identity crisis: Global challenges of identity protection in a

    This paper follows in the footsteps of 'FIDIS' (The Future of Identity in the Information Society), an EU-funded network of excellence from 2004 to 2009 that provided an integrated inter-disciplinary approach to an identity research agenda, by offering a broad overview for possible reflection on how future legal research around digital ...

  8. Fifty Years Since "Identity: Youth and Crisis": A Renewed Look at

    This special issue of Identity was inspired by the semicentennial of Erikson's landmark book "Identity: Youth and Crisis," published in Citation 1968.Erikson wrote on his concept of identity both before and after 1968. This particular book, mostly comprising previously published essays, was not compiled in order to present a systematically organized exposition of the concept of identity ...

  9. The impact of the identity crisis on the adjustment of late adolescent

    The relationship between ego identity status and MMPI scores was investigated using 73 college males. Identity status was assessed using a semistructured interview. The results obtained indicated that subjects in the identity achievement status obtained MMPI scores which fell within the normal range of functioning. Subjects currently involved in working on identity issues (moratorium status ...

  10. Dynamics of Identity Development in Adolescence: A Decade in Review

    In the current review, we discuss progress in the field of identity research between 2010 and 2020. One of the key developmental tasks in adolescence and young adulthood is to develop a coherent sense of self and identity (Erikson, 1968).Personal identity refers to one's sense of the person one genuinely is, including a subjective feeling of self‐sameness and continuity over contexts and time.

  11. Links of Adolescents Identity Development and Relationship with Peers

    According to Erik Erikson, the main task of adolescents is to solve the crisis of identity versus role confusion. Research has shown that a stable and strong sense of identity is associated with better mental health of adolescents. Good relationships with peers are also linked with better emotional and psychological well-being of adolescents.

  12. [PDF] Comparative Study of Identity Crisis in Students based on Age

    Identity crisis means uncertainty and ambiguity in a person's definition of self and his or her being, and it prevents him or her to have an integrated character. This research aims to study the identity crisis in students comparatively based on age, sex, and level of education. The study is descriptive and correlation type. The statistical population includes all students of Lorestan ...

  13. PDF The Crisis of Identity

    The Crisis of Identity. Traditional analysis of humanitarian assistance and interventions focuses on issues pertaining to human rights violations, social and economic sustainability, and political stability and security. The politics of assistance have failed to develop strong intervention mechanisms that treat issues of identity crisis as an ...

  14. PDF Identity Crisis: a Search of Self

    Research Paper *Corresponding Author: S.Rajeshwari 59 | Page Identity Crisis: a Search of Self S.Rajeshwari (Department of English, GayatriVidhyaParishad College of Engineering for Women) ... The concept, identity crisis was developed by ErikErikson who believed that the formation of identity was one of the most important parts in one‟s life.

  15. (DOC) Identity Crisis: A study on the different perceptions of

    Academia.edu is a platform for academics to share research papers. Identity Crisis: A study on the different perceptions of Filipinos on a concrete national identity (2015) ... Appendix Graph 1: Percentage of the top 300 comments in their respective categories 18 16 | Identity Crisis: A study on the different perceptions of Filipinos on a ...

  16. (PDF) Critical Analysis of Identity Crisis in Hanif Kurieshi's Novel

    Postcolonial theoretical ideas i.e, hybridity, mimicry, assimilation, and ambivalence by Homi K. Bhabha are applied into the text of this research paper to examine the dilemma of identity crisis more clearly. Karim Amir, the protagonist of the novel faces an identity crisis in tormenting and perturbing the social order of England.

  17. PDF Existential Dilemma In Indian Women With Special Reference To Identity

    Research Paper Existential Dilemma In Indian Women With Special Reference To Identity Crisis: A Study of Anita Desai's Cry, The Peacock And Fire On The Mountain And Jhumpa Lahiri's The Namesake And Interpreter Of Maladies Atashi Ghosh (Assistant Teacher in R.S.B.Vidyayatan, Raina, Burdwan, West Bengal) Corresponding author: Atashi Ghosh

  18. Realizing R&B's Identity Crisis: An Analytical Research Paper

    More specifically, this paper will use qualitative and historical research methods to evaluate how R&B is affected by genre interplay, racial issues, and the rise of alternative R&B—in an attempt to gain a further understanding of the genre's current identity crisis and highlight areas for future research.