Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here .

Loading metrics

Open Access

Peer-reviewed

Research Article

Attitudes on voluntary and mandatory vaccination against COVID-19: Evidence from Germany

Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

Affiliation DIW Berlin / SOEP, Berlin, Germany

ORCID logo

* E-mail: [email protected] (CSP); [email protected] (CS)

Affiliation Karlsruhe Institute of Technology, Karlsruhe, Germany

Affiliations DIW Berlin / SOEP, Berlin, Germany, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany

  • Daniel Graeber, 
  • Christoph Schmidt-Petri, 
  • Carsten Schröder

PLOS

  • Published: May 10, 2021
  • https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248372
  • Reader Comments

Table 1

Several vaccines against COVID-19 have now been developed and are already being rolled out around the world. The decision whether or not to get vaccinated has so far been left to the individual citizens. However, there are good reasons, both in theory as well as in practice, to believe that the willingness to get vaccinated might not be sufficiently high to achieve herd immunity. A policy of mandatory vaccination could ensure high levels of vaccination coverage, but its legitimacy is doubtful. We investigate the willingness to get vaccinated and the reasons for an acceptance (or rejection) of a policy of mandatory vaccination against COVID-19 in June and July 2020 in Germany based on a representative real time survey, a random sub-sample (SOEP-CoV) of the German Socio-Economic Panel (SOEP). Our results show that about 70 percent of adults in Germany would voluntarily get vaccinated against the coronavirus if a vaccine without side effects was available. About half of residents of Germany are in favor, and half against, a policy of mandatory vaccination. The approval rate for mandatory vaccination is significantly higher among those who would get vaccinated voluntarily (around 60 percent) than among those who would not get vaccinated voluntarily (27 percent). The individual willingness to get vaccinated and acceptance of a policy of mandatory vaccination correlates systematically with socio-demographic and psychological characteristics of the respondents. We conclude that as far as people’s declared intentions are concerned, herd immunity could be reached without a policy of mandatory vaccination, but that such a policy might be found acceptable too, were it to become necessary.

Citation: Graeber D, Schmidt-Petri C, Schröder C (2021) Attitudes on voluntary and mandatory vaccination against COVID-19: Evidence from Germany. PLoS ONE 16(5): e0248372. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248372

Editor: Valerio Capraro, Middlesex University, UNITED KINGDOM

Received: October 19, 2020; Accepted: February 25, 2021; Published: May 10, 2021

Copyright: © 2021 Graeber et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Data Availability: Our analyses rely on the German Socio-Economic Panel (SOEP), an independent scientific data infrastructure established in 1984. We, as users, cannot send the data to the journal and make them publicly available, as this is against SOEP's statutes (and most likely against the statutes of all providers of micro data). However, this should not be a hurdle, as researchers from scientific institutions around the globe can access the data (free of costs) once they have signed a user contract. The scientific use file of the SOEP with anonymous microdata is made available free of charge to universities and research institutes for research and teaching purposes. The direct use of SOEP data is subject to the provisions of German data protection law. Therefore, signing a data distribution contract is the single precondition for working with SOEP data. The data distribution contract can be requested with a form which can be downloaded from: http://www.diw.de/documents/dokumentenarchiv/17/diw_01.c.88926.de/soep_application_contract.pdf .

Funding: The data collection of the SOEP-CoV Study was financially supported by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research. We acknowledge support by the KIT-Publication Fund of the Karlsruhe Institute of Technology. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Introduction

Great efforts have been made worldwide to develop a vaccine against COVID-19. When we first drafted this article, in October 2020, 35 different potential vaccines were in clinical trials and 145 were still in the pre-clinical stage. In February 2021, several vaccines have been approved in many countries and are being rolled out, 74 are in clinical trials, and 182 are in the pre-clinical stage [ 1 ].

These developments are very encouraging, as a wide availability of vaccines is seen by many as a prerequisite for a return to a “normal” pre-COVID-19 type of social and economic life. With the growing availability of vaccines comes the hope that coercive measures such as restrictions on international trade, contact restrictions, and travel bans, etc., which cause enormous economic and social costs, may soon be removed and will not need to be reimplemented.

Of course, any vaccine is only an effective contribution to a return to normal life if a sufficiently high number of people are actually vaccinated, yielding herd immunity. If so, vaccination secures a public good: protection from COVID-19 for everyone. From a microeconomic perspective, this raises a well-known problem, free-riding: If the vaccination is freely available but not obligatory, then citizens’ individual decisions determine the extent to which this public good is made available. In order to make that decision, they will weigh their own costs and benefits. These costs include the time sacrificed, physical unpleasantness, possible side effects of a vaccination, etc. The benefits to a particular individual are primarily, but not necessarily exclusively, the reduction in risk to that person’s own health or material well-being. From a welfare perspective, if individuals do not take into account the positive externalities on third parties that their own vaccination triggers, there will be an undersupply of the public good. Following [ 2 , 3 ], individuals’ utility function may also include other-regarding preferences and hence yield a direct benefit from contributions to a public good. In our context, people could therefore benefit from a ‘warm glow of vaccinating’, because by vaccinating themselves they also reduce the risks of others. But even so there is certainly no guarantee that the social optimum will be reached [ 4 ] or that a sufficiently high number of people will freely choose to get vaccinated.

It is frequently argued that vaccination should be made mandatory because of the free-rider problem [ 5 ]: While vaccinated individuals have incurred private costs in terms of discomfort or money and receive the private benefit of a reduced risk of getting the disease, the major collective benefit, the reduced incidence of disease, is public. If enough other people produce the public benefit, and the circulation of the virus decreases accordingly, an individual might rationally decide to free-ride on others’ decisions. A policy of mandatory vaccination would prevent this.

[ 6 ] argue that such a policy would not be necessary: “If vaccinations are perfect, then if one is vaccinated he or she does not care whether others are vaccinated, so there is no longer any public good problem” ([ 6 ], p. 70). Hence there would not be a case in favor of mandatory vaccination, as under such a policy, individuals who would have favored not to be vaccinated are made worse off, while those who anyway would get vaccinated are not better off.

However, by definition, ‘perfect’ vaccination means that everyone vaccinated is perfectly immune [ 6 ]. In the current situation, it can neither be taken for granted that a perfect vaccination is being or will be provided soon, nor that everyone who wants to also will have the possibility to be vaccinated (both financially and in terms of health). If perfect vaccination is not feasible, however, mandatory vaccination is not dominated by a laissez faire solution [ 6 ].

Extensions of this theoretical public good analysis emphasize the relevance of behavioral aspects not typically considered in classical models. The empirical literature also highlights a number of factors that matter for vaccine uptake. For instance [ 7 ], show that social norms matter for an individual’s willingness to get a vaccination and that such norms can suppress vaccine uptake even in the presence of frequent disease outbreaks. Further [ 8 ], show that the design of public vaccination policies should also take intergroup interactions into account. Other-regarding preferences can explain voluntary vaccination uptake, as argued by [ 9 ]. For example [ 10 ], show that the presence of individuals who cannot get vaccinated, like babies and the elderly, increases the willingness to get vaccinated. The static model in [ 6 ] also does not reflect interactive processes [ 9 , 11 ]. show that vaccination is the individually best response until a certain vaccination rate is reached in the population and becomes a social dilemma only from this vaccination rate until herd immunity is maximized. Communicating the social benefits of vaccination can have positive effects, particularly when this protects vulnerable groups, but it can also invite free-riding [ 12 ]. Those people who cannot get vaccinated themselves for medical reasons are particularly vulnerable: they cannot protect themselves even if they wanted to and, hence, depend on their fellow citizens to protect them by preventing the spread of the virus through their vaccination. Children, too, need to be considered separately. Since they cannot give informed consent to a voluntary vaccination themselves, they might have to be protected from their parents (who might be unwilling to get them vaccinated) in case of particularly serious diseases (see [ 13 , 14 ]).

There is, in summary, hope that the public goods problem may be overcome, as social and behavioral science offers a wide array of potential policy options to influence people’s perceptions and reactions to the pandemic (for an extensive up-to-date overview, see [ 15 ]). It is not clear, however, how the research on well-established vaccines carries over to the current pandemic, and recent developments seem to indicate that the willingness to get vaccinated against the novel coronavirus is currently rather low. We therefore chose to investigate two fundamental questions at the opposite extremes of the spectrum of policy options: would a sufficient number of people voluntarily undergo vaccination to achieve herd immunity? Or would a mandatory vaccination against COVID-19 be acceptable to achieve herd immunity?

A legal duty to be vaccinated against COVID-19 could be an alternative to other coercive measures if one assumes that a high-risk, unregulated, laissez faire approach is not a realistic policy option: it seems irresponsible to lift all restrictions because the virus would soon spread through the entire population. Coercive measures of some kind therefore seem inevitable. Mandatory vaccination could be preferable to other coercive measures, provided the interference with bodily integrity would be considered less socially costly in the long run than the effects of prolonged lockdowns. Emotions run high where vaccination policies are concerned, but because mandatory vaccination might become a realistic scenario, it is worth investigating what the general population thinks about such a policy.

It is important to emphasize that a legal duty to vaccinate against COVID-19 would not imply a legal (or even moral) duty to vaccinate against other diseases. The novel coronavirus is a special case in many respects: In contrast to influenza, for example, the population does not have a background immunity from past infections. In addition, many infected people do not show symptoms (a recent meta-study estimates this to be one in six infected [ 16 ]) and, hence, cannot protect others from being infected through voluntary self-quarantining. Thus, people with COVID-19 represent a much higher risk of infection for others than, for example, people who come down with influenza, assuming that these would normally stay at home. Therefore, a vaccination against COVID-19 is much more important from the social perspective than e.g. a vaccination against influenza: not for self-protection, but to protect other people from unintentional infection. Although classic liberal positions (cf. [ 17 ]) would reject a paternalist legal obligation to protect oneself through vaccination, they plausibly would favor a policy of mandatory vaccination in the case of COVID-19 to protect others from being harmed. In modern philosophical discussions, even some libertarians are in favor of mandatory vaccination against serious diseases for similar reasons (see [ 18 ] and for an overview [ 19 ]).

Though there are philosophical reasons supporting a policy of mandatory vaccination, we want to emphasize that we are not advocating it as a concrete policy option for Germany at this moment. Our aim is to understand whether the general public would consider such a policy acceptable, or which sections of the population, and why. To this end, we study the willingness to get vaccinated and the acceptance of a policy of mandatory vaccination against COVID-19 in June and July 2020 in Germany. We use unique real time survey data from a sub-sample (SOEP-CoV) of the German Socio-Economic Panel (SOEP, see [ 20 ]). A set of questions about vaccination was part of the later stages of SOEP-CoV, an ongoing research project initiated in April 2020. This so-called ‘vaccination module’ included questions on the willingness to get vaccinated voluntarily and the acceptance of a policy of mandatory vaccination against COVID-19. In addition, individuals could indicate reasons for their preference regarding the second question. Using the rich data of the SOEP, pre-pandemic income, education, household context, personality, political preferences etc., which can be directly linked with SOEP-CoV, we are able to provide a detailed picture on who intends to get vaccinated and who does not.

The most important result of our study is that about 70 percent of adults in Germany would get vaccinated voluntarily against COVID-19 if a vaccine without significant side effects was available. Further, about half of adults in Germany are in favor, and half against, a policy of mandatory vaccination against COVID-19. The approval rate for mandatory vaccination is significantly higher among those who would get vaccinated voluntarily (around 60 percent) than among those who would not get vaccinated voluntarily (27 percent). However, 22 percent of the individuals would disapprove of both a voluntary and a mandatory vaccination and 8 percent can be characterized as ‘passengers’ (they are not willing to get vaccinated but do support a policy of mandatory vaccination, but they might not all be ‘free-riders’ in the standard sense). In this group, surprisingly, 86 percent state that, without a mandatory vaccination, too few individuals would get vaccinated and about 87 percent indicate that most people underestimate how dangerous COVID-19 is. In general, the willingness to get vaccinated is significantly lower for female, younger, and less educated respondents as well as those with lower income. A policy of mandatory vaccination is rejected with higher probability by women and favored by older people and those living in the eastern federal states.

Data, measures, and methods

Data: soep and soep-cov.

The German Socio-economic Panel (SOEP) is among the largest and longest-running representative panel surveys worldwide and is recognized for maintaining the highest standards of data quality and research ethics [ 20 ]. In 2020, the survey covers about 30,000 adults in 20,000 households. Since the same individuals and households participate in the study every year, life courses of the respondents can be tracked and intertemporal analyses can be carried out at the individual and at the household level. The data contain information on the respondents’ household situation, education, labor market outcomes, and health, among others (see [ 20 , 21 ]).

To better understand the effects of the corona pandemic, a special survey called SOEP-CoV was conducted within the framework of the SOEP, which consisted of a random sample of about 6,700 SOEP respondents, (see [ 21 , 22 ]). SOEP-CoV was surveyed in nine staggered tranches from early April to the end of July 2020 and collected data on the following topics: a) Prevalence, health behavior, and health inequality; b) Labor market and gainful employment; c) Social life, networks, and mobility; d) Mental health and well-being; and e) Social cohesion. Over time, some new question modules were introduced within these five thematic complexes. These included the ‘vaccination module’ (see questionnaires available under www.soep-cov.de/Methodik/ ).

Measures: Preferences toward vaccination against COVID-19

The ‘vaccination module’ went into the field with tranches 7 to 9, in June and July 2020, and covered a total of 851 persons aged 19 years and older. At that moment, major research efforts were being undertaken, but it was not clear whether any vaccine would actually be found. The module hence starts with a question on the hypothetical willingness to get vaccinated against COVID-19:

  • “Let us assume that a vaccine against the novel coronavirus that is shown to have no significant side effects is found. Would you get vaccinated?” The response categories are ’Yes’, ’No’, and ‘no answer’. The module contains a further question about mandatory vaccination with the same response categories:
  • “Would you be in favor of a policy of mandatory vaccination against the coronavirus?” In addition, the interviewees were asked about their reasons for or against a policy of mandatory vaccination. For this purpose, a filter was used to adapt the arguments according to the respondents’ answers to question (B). The arguments given were as follows:

Argument 1: Others’ willingness to get vaccinated without mandatory vaccination

  • Against mandatory vaccination: “Enough people would get vaccinated even without a policy of mandatory vaccination.”
  • In favor of mandatory vaccination: “Only with a policy of mandatory vaccination would enough people get vaccinated.”

Argument 2: Misperception of risks

  • Against mandatory vaccination: “Most people overestimate the dangerousness of the virus.”
  • In favor of mandatory vaccination: “Most people underestimate the dangerousness of the virus.”

Argument 3: Legitimacy of a policy of mandatory vaccinations in general

  • Against mandatory vaccination: “A policy of mandatory vaccination is never permissible, even in the case of very dangerous diseases.”
  • In favor of mandatory vaccination: “A policy of mandatory vaccinations would make sense also for less dangerous diseases.”

Argument 4: Other reasons (without listing these reasons explicitly)

The first three arguments are of particular relevance for political decision-making. Although there is quite a lot of research on the reasons people have not to get vaccinated themselves, there is much less research on what people think about policies of mandatory vaccinations, and up to present–at least to our knowledge–none on the application to the special case of the novel coronavirus. As the reasons for the individual decision need not carry over to the policy assessment, and given the previously discussed particularities of the coronavirus, we focused on factors that are both of theoretical importance and under discussion in the general public. It would be interesting, for instance, if many people did not have the intention to get vaccinated themselves, yet believed that enough other people would get vaccinated so that mandatory vaccination would not be required. Similarly, it would be surprising if people wanted to get vaccinated yet believed that others overestimated the dangerousness of the virus. Finally, we wanted to see whether people considered mandatory vaccinations potentially legitimate at all.

Sample selection, weighting, and item non-response

Since SOEP-CoV is a random sample from the SOEP population, the SOEP-CoV data 2020 can be linked with the regular SOEP data of previous years. Thus, attitudes toward vaccination against COVID-19 that were collected during the pandemic can be linked to the characteristics of the respondents before the outbreak of the pandemic (e.g., income or educational level). Since these characteristics were collected before the pandemic, they can be considered unaffected by the pandemic event and, hence, exogenous ( S1 File provides definitions of all dependent and independent variables used in the empirical analyses).

The response rate in the vaccination module was high. Altogether, only 4.58 percent of the 851 respondents did not answer the question about voluntary vaccination and 3.41 percent did not answer the question about mandatory vaccination. Of those who supported (objected to) mandatory vaccination, 0.26 (1.82) percent did not provide at least one motive in the follow-up question. Hence, bias from item non-response should be small and we did not correct for it. As the focal variables are coded dichotomously (yes = 1; no = 0), there was no need to remove outliers in them from the database.

To derive population-wide estimates, the SOEP-CoV data is equipped with frequency weights. The weighting of SOEP-CoV follows the standard weighting used in SOEP [ 23 , 24 ]. Based on the SOEP household weights, weights for all persons in the participating households were generated via a marginal adjustment step and corrected for selection effects. Furthermore, the data were corrected for the fact that some SOEP subsamples were excluded from the SOEP-CoV study from the outset. To address potential selection effects and adjust frequency weights accordingly, we followed the two-step procedure recommended in [ 25 ]:

  • Step 1: Estimation of a logistic regression model where the dependent variable is a dummy variable indicating whether respondents belong to the working sample of tranches 7 to 9 (dummy is equal to one) or not (dummy is zero). All variables included in the following analyses serve as explanatory variables.
  • Step 2: If at least one analysis variable shows a significant (i.e., p -value below 0.05) and at the same time meaningful effect (i.e., coefficient above 0.01) with respect to the assignment to the analysis population, a correction of the SOEP-CoV weights is performed by multiplying the frequency weights by the inverse estimated probability. In other words, multiplying the SOEP-CoV weights belonging to the analysis set by the inverse predicted probability yields the sought adjusted weight that can be used to calculate population statistics. In the present case, an adjustment using the following variables is indicated: Extraversion and whether respondents live in a household in which at least one household member was tested for COVID-19. Overall, selection on observables is very minor. Unless otherwise stated, our results are weighted with the adjusted probability weights.

Statistical framework

Since the vaccination questions are answered once by each respondent, our empirical strategy is between-person. Uni- and bivariate results for our focal variable, attitudes toward vaccination, are presented as weighted means or percentages. Assessments of differences in attitudes or characteristics between-groups rely on two-tailed t-tests, with statistical significance evaluated at p <0.01, p <0.05, and p <0.10 using the survey weights explained above. Our empirical strategy involves multiple between-group tests. This raises the question of whether a correction is necessary for multiple hypotheses testing. We do not implement such a correction because we seek to compare a certain attitude or characteristic between groups and not to draw, at the end of the test series, a concluding summary of all tests results.

argumentative research paper on vaccines

Willingness to get vaccinated and attitudes toward a policy of mandatory vaccination

For the questions on voluntary vaccination (A) and mandatory vaccination (B), four groups in the population may be distinguished:

  • Anti-vaccination: interviewees who would not get vaccinated voluntarily against the coronavirus and who also oppose a policy of mandatory vaccination.
  • Anti-duty: interviewees who would get vaccinated voluntarily but oppose a policy of mandatory vaccination.
  • Passengers: interviewees who would not get vaccinated voluntarily but are in favor of mandatory vaccination. We refer to this group as ‘passengers’ because they apparently want to see the public good of herd immunity provided by mandatory vaccination, yet would not voluntarily contribute to this good. Some of these passengers might be free-riders in the standard sense, trying to benefit from the decisions of others while not voluntarily contributing themselves, while others might not be able to get vaccinated for medical reasons. If mandatory vaccination were introduced, the first group, but not the second, would also get vaccinated, of course. Neither group would actually free-ride, but the first might initially have wanted to.
  • Pro-vaccination: interviewees who would get vaccinated voluntarily and are also in favor of mandatory vaccination.

Overall, 70 percent of adults in Germany would voluntarily get vaccinated against the coronavirus, provided a vaccine without significant side effects was available ( Table 1 : groups 2 and 4). This value corresponds exactly to the results of [ 26 ]. From May till September 2020, the COVID-19 snapshot monitoring (COSMO) at the University of Erfurt showed relatively constant values of between 60 and 66 percent; it was only in April that it showed an exceptionally high value of 79 percent, and it has now decreased further (cf. [ 27 ], p. 76; an overview of previous studies on the willingness to get vaccinated in Germany is provided in S2 File .). Overall, these studies paint a consistent picture, with a slight decline in the willingness in the second half of 2020.

thumbnail

  • PPT PowerPoint slide
  • PNG larger image
  • TIFF original image

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248372.t001

Approximately half of the interviewees are against, and half are in favor of, a policy of mandatory vaccination (against: 51%, groups 1 and 2, in favor: 49%, groups 3 and 4). These values, too, coincide almost exactly with those of the COSMO monitoring since May 2020 (cf. [ 27 ], p. 78), which in April showed an approval rate for mandatory vaccination of 73 percent, but later discontinued this question (till July 2020, and it has been decreasing since; see S2 File ). The agreement with a policy of mandatory vaccination is clearly higher, namely almost 60 percent (41/(41+29) = 0.59) among those who would get vaccinated voluntarily than with those who would not let themselves be vaccinated voluntarily, i.e. approximately 27 percent (8/(8+22) = 0.27).

Attitudes toward a policy of mandatory vaccination

The four groups differ noticeably in how they assess the arguments presented to them. This is shown in Table 2 , which gives the group-specific approval rate for each argument in combination with the p-values of t-tests in S3.1 Table in S3 File .

thumbnail

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248372.t002

Argument 1.

The groups differ markedly in how likely they think it is that others will get vaccinated. Among the two groups that are against a policy of mandatory vaccination, 56 percent of the ‘anti-vaccination’ group (who would not get vaccinated voluntarily) think that their fellow citizens would get vaccinated sufficiently frequently such that mandatory vaccinations would not be necessary. Almost 80 percent of the ‘anti-duty’ group (the members of which would get vaccinated voluntarily) think the same. Among the two groups that are in favor of mandatory vaccination, 85 percent of the ‘passengers’ (who would not voluntarily get vaccinated) think that the others would not voluntarily get vaccinated either, as do slightly more than 90 percent of the ‘pro-vaccination’ group (who would also get vaccinated voluntarily).

Argument 2.

These results run in parallel with the assessment of the dangerousness of the virus. Even though the analysis is not causal, we can see that about 50 percent of the ‘anti-vaccination’ group and 30 percent of the ‘anti-duty’ group think that most people overestimate the dangerousness of SARS-CoV-2. Exactly the opposite, that most people underestimate the dangerousness, is believed by nearly 90 percent of the ‘passenger’ group and by slightly more than 80 percent of the ‘pro-vaccination’ group. Summarizing the numbers differently, one could say that groups 2 and 4, who would voluntarily get vaccinated, probably have similar opinions about whether their fellow citizens correctly assess the danger posed by the virus. About 80 percent of the members of the ‘pro-vaccination’ group think that most people underestimate the danger. Of the members of the ‘anti-duty’ group, we only know with certainty that 30 percent of them believe that most people overestimate the danger–we do not know, however, how the remaining 70 percent are divided between ’underestimate’ and ’correctly estimate’. The difference between the corresponding values for groups 1 and 3 is significantly higher.

Looking at arguments 1 and 2, we may conclude that there is a high level of disagreement among the population about the dangerousness of the virus. This disagreement probably explains why people have such different attitudes toward getting vaccinated and toward the necessity (or not) of a policy of mandatory vaccination.

The position of the group of the ‘passengers’ is hard to understand. On the one hand, they favor a policy of mandatory vaccination, presumably because, as they do believe, the dangerousness of the virus is often underestimated. On the other hand, they probably assume that they themselves do not underestimate that dangerousness, but nevertheless would not get vaccinated voluntarily. One reason for this could be their medical condition: they might be willing but unable to get vaccinated for medical reasons. If so, they would not be trying to free-ride. It is unclear, however, how much weight the appeal to such a hypothetical medical contraindication should have, given that at the time of the interviews, no vaccine was even available. Some, but not all, of the ‘passengers’ are probably free-riders in the standard sense.

Argument 3.

Approximately 40 percent of both the ‘anti-vaccination’ group and the ‘anti-duty’ group agree with the statement that a mandatory vaccination is never permissible, not even with very dangerous diseases. Since these two groups reject mandatory vaccination against COVID-19, this means that for the remaining 60 percent of the group, mandatory vaccination may well be permissible–but apparently only for diseases that they would have to consider as even more dangerous than COVID-19. Conversely, well over 60 percent of the ‘passenger’ group and just over 70 percent of the ‘pro-vaccination’ group agree with the statement that a policy of mandatory vaccination would also make sense for less dangerous diseases. In combination with the results for argument 2, these two groups could therefore believe that their fellow citizens also underestimate the danger of such other diseases. It is interesting to note that, overall, people in Germany estimate the probability that the novel coronavirus will cause a life-threatening disease within the next twelve months to be high (cf. [ 20 ]). This probability is around 25 percent across our four groups. In group 1 it is 20 percent, in group 2 around 27 percent, in group 3 it is 30 percent and in group 4 it is 25 percent (see Table 3 ).

thumbnail

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248372.t003

Other reasons (which were not further broken down in the questionnaire for capacity reasons) are important primarily among those respondents who would not themselves get vaccinated and also oppose mandatory vaccination. Although questions (A) and (B) explicitly assume that a vaccine would not have any significant side effects, this could be due to a deeper skepticism about vaccination, which we hope to be able to explore in future research (on ’vaccine denialism’ see [ 28 ]).

Characteristics of the ‘anti-vaccination’, ‘anti-duty’, ‘passenger’, and ‘pro-vaccination’ groups

Description of the individual characteristics of the groups..

We would like to know in more detail who is in favor of a policy of mandatory vaccination against COVID-19 and who is opposing it, as well as what the socio-economic characteristics of those who would get vaccinated and of those who would not are. Table 3 shows how the four groups defined above differ across various socio-demographic characteristics (measured before the pandemic), personality (measured before the pandemic), health (before and during the pandemic), and political orientation (measured before the pandemic). Statistical t-tests for the significance of differences in characteristics between groups are shown in S3.2 Table in S3 File assuming equal variances across groups. S3.3 Table in S3 File provides supporting evidence: tests for equality of variance across groups provides support for this assumption in about 90% of the cases, and as S3.4 Table in S3 File . shows, relaxing the equality of variances assumption does not change our conclusions.

Socio-demographic characteristics . Almost 60 percent of the ‘anti-vaccination’ group are female, they are on average 48 years old, 12 percent of them have a university degree and their monthly net household income in 2019 averaged just under EUR 2,800. Around 27 percent have children under 16 and around 17 percent live in the eastern German states. ‘Passengers’ do not differ in their characteristics statistically significantly from this group. The members of the ‘anti-duty’ group, by contrast, are much more likely to be male and more often have a university degree. In comparison to the ‘anti-vaccination’ group, the members of the ‘pro-vaccination’ group are also more often male and older, and are also more likely to have a university degree. In particular, older interviewees are more likely to be in groups that favor mandatory vaccination and persons with a university education in groups comprising those who would get vaccinated voluntarily.

Personality traits . SOEP collects the personality traits of the respondents using a battery of questions that measure the five dimensions of the so-called ’Big Five’ [ 29 ]. The Big Five are the five most important groups of character traits in personality research: ’openness’, ’conscientiousness’, ’extraversion (sociability)’, ’tolerance’, and ’neuroticism’. Furthermore, risk attitude is surveyed. We see that members of the ‘anti-vaccination’ group tend to be more sociable but less open than the other groups. Their willingness to take risks is similar to that of the members of the ‘anti-duty’ and of the ‘pro-vaccination’ groups, but is significantly higher than that of ‘passengers’. Members of the ‘anti-duty’ group are particularly unsociable compared to the other groups, but open to new experiences. The ‘passengers’ are, like the members of the ‘pro-vaccination’ group, less neurotic. They are particularly tolerable and the least willing to take risks of the four groups.

Health . As far as the health of those surveyed is concerned, statistically significant differences are only evident in the number of illnesses: Members of the ‘anti-vaccination’ group have significantly fewer risk diseases than ‘passengers’ and members of the ‘pro-vaccination’ group. ‘Anti-duty’ members, on the other hand, have significantly fewer diseases than the ‘passengers’. Thus, overall, it may be said that those who refuse a policy of mandatory vaccination have fewer risk diseases at the time of the survey. There are no differences between the groups in terms of whether a member of the respondent’s household has already undergone a test for an infection with corona. It should be noted, however, that the number of cases of those tested for an infection is comparatively small.

Political orientation . As far as the political orientation of the respondents is concerned, no systematic significant differences between the four groups are identified. Only the members of the ‘anti-vaccination’ group seem to be positioned somewhat more to the right in the party spectrum than the members of the ‘anti-duty’ group.

Multivariate description of the characteristics of the four groups.

The differences and similarities with regard to group composition described above always refer to a single characteristic, i.e. they are univariate. Additionally, the relationships between individual characteristics of the respondents–after taking other characteristics into account–and their attitude toward mandatory or voluntary vaccination are explained below using a multivariate model (logistic estimation; see Eq ( 1 )). The dependent variable is either an indicator that describes the respondent’s own willingness to get vaccinated voluntarily (value 1 = yes; 0 = no; Table 4 ) or an indicator (value 1 = yes; 0 = no) that describes whether the respondents favors a policy of mandatory vaccination ( Table 5 ). As our interest is the explanation of data structures, we do not use survey weights in the multivariate analysis.

thumbnail

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248372.t004

thumbnail

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248372.t005

With regard to the willingness to voluntarily get vaccinated ( Table 4 ), some significant differences in socio-demographic characteristics are observed. If all other characteristics are kept constant, the willingness to vaccinate is about 10 percentage points lower in women than in men. It is positively associated with age (0.4 percentage points per year of life), education (13 percentage points if respondents have a university degree compared to the other education categories), and household income (2.5 percentage points per 1,000 euros). The personality traits of the Big Five do not correlate with the respondents’ willingness to vaccinate; only openness is slightly positively associated with the willingness to vaccinate. In the health block, there is also only one significant variable that correlates with the willingness to get vaccinated: The higher the respondents estimate the probability that the virus could trigger a life-threatening disease in them, the more willing they are to be vaccinated.

A policy of mandatory vaccination ( Table 5 ) is also rejected with higher probability by women, but favored by older people and those living in the eastern federal states, ceteris paribus . Approval is negatively associated with neuroticism, i.e. emotional instability, and positively associated with the subjective probability of contracting life-threatening COVID-19.

The tables presenting the logit estimations include an initial model diagnostic: the Pseudo- R 2 . In S4 File , we present two additional model diagnostics: First, the linktest for both logit models does not find any evidence for model misspecifications. Second, a receiver operator characteristic (ROC) analysis provides evidence that the predictive power of our two models is acceptable. In addition, to assess multicollinearity, we have computed variance inflation factors (VIF) in S5 File . As a rule of thumb, a variable whose VIF values exceeds 10 may merit further investigation. In both regressions, the VIF of none of the explanatory variable exceeds 7.7 and the average VIF over all variables is below 2.1. It should also be noted that the two separate logit models do not model correlation and heteroscedasticity between the two outcomes (vaccinate voluntarily or obligatorily). Hence, in S6 File , as a robustness check, we have estimated a multivariate probit model using Stata’s mvprobit command that relies on simulated maximum likelihood [ 30 ]. S6.1-S6.6 Tables in S6 File compare the coefficients of the multivariate probit with the two separate models. Overall, there are some changes in the magnitude of the coefficients, but no changes in the signs of the regression coefficients or significance levels.

It is possible that respondents who did not give an answer about their vaccination preferences–for example, because they are still undecided–would decide to vaccinate or support mandatory vaccination after an adequate vaccination campaign. In a robustness check, we followed this argument by assigning respondents who refused to answer the question about voluntary or mandatory vaccination to the ‘yes’ category and repeated the logit estimation. This does not change our results (see S7.1 and S7.2 Tables in S7 File , S8 File provides our Stata code, which prepares the data and conducts all the statistical analyses, as well as the outputs of the multivariate estimations).

Finally, Table 6 provides a statistical comparison of the marginal effects from the model on willingness to get vaccinated ( Table 4 ) and attitudes toward mandatory vaccinations ( Table 5 ). We find no significant differences in marginal effects between the two models except for two variables: tertiary education and eastern federal states. The marginal effect for tertiary education is significantly larger for the willingness to get vaccinated model while the opposite is true for eastern federal states.

thumbnail

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248372.t006

Politicians must make decisions which are based on incomplete information yet have far-reaching consequences for public health, personal freedom, and economic prosperity. It seems that many citizens are prepared to behave responsibly in the sense that they are prepared to endure a ‘little sting’ for the good of all: a vast majority of the German population (70 percent) state that they would get vaccinated as soon as a vaccine against COVID-19 was available. This means that under favorable conditions, a legal duty to get vaccinated to achieve herd immunity might not be necessary. It should, however, be noted that the question was asked in a stylized context: Potential side effects or ineffectiveness of the then hypothetical vaccine were assumed away. Though there is no reason to believe the vaccines currently being administered are more problematic in this respect than any other vaccines in use, neither can strictly be guaranteed in reality. In addition, the time required for a vaccination, the process of the vaccination itself (i.e. the injection), bureaucratic administration (e.g. making an appointment with the family doctor) or any necessary co-payment should de facto reduce the willingness to get vaccinated. Furthermore, at the time of writing, not only is it still unclear how quickly a vaccine can even be produced in the quantity required and administered to enough people, it is also unclear how long its effect will last. It is not even clear what percentage of the population would have to be vaccinated to achieve herd immunity, as this also depends on individual behavior and legal (or ethical) norms which are likely to continue to change (e.g. an explicit or implicit obligation to wear a mask of a specific variety in public transport or a testing obligation for people returning from trips abroad) [ 31 , 32 ]. Hence, a sufficiently high willingness to get vaccinated in the ‘best case’ scenario investigated here is an idealization and in any case only one relevant factor among many.

We observed there to be gender differences in the willingness to get vaccinated: women are less willing to get vaccinated, and also less willing to support a policy of mandatory vaccination. This is surprising, given that men are generally less likely to engage in preventive behavior [ 33 ] and women have been shown to be more willing to engage in preventive behavior in the pandemic, for instance by wearing face masks when recommended [ 34 ], and they also seem to be more compliant with other measures in general [ 35 ]. However, men are also more severely affected by the coronavirus [ 36 ] and women generally more skeptical about vaccinations, especially against COVID-19 [ 37 ]. We don’t know whether our interviewees frame their decision to get vaccinated or not as a situation of a social dilemma, but if so, previous results on gender differences in cooperation suggest men and women might have to be addressed differently to influence their decisions [ 38 , 39 ]. We also observed that income and education increase the willingness to get vaccinated voluntarily. It has also been shown recently that the willingness to pay for a vaccine against Covid-19 is positively impacted by, among other variables, income [ 40 ].

A mandatory vaccination would almost certainly achieve herd immunity against COVID-19, since all those for whom there is no medical contraindication would also get vaccinated. About half of the respondents approve and disapprove, respectively, of such a mandatory vaccination policy. In this context, the strong disagreement among the participants of the study regarding the dangerousness of the virus is particularly striking. Many of those who reject a policy of mandatory vaccination assume the dangerousness of the virus is being overestimated by others, while those who approve of a policy of mandatory vaccination seem to believe the exact opposite. This is highly problematic: at most one of the two groups can be right. Plausibly, the interviewees themselves differ in how dangerous they think the virus is. This yields a concrete and important policy recommendation (see also [ 40 , 41 ]): we need more reliable data on the dangerousness of SARS-CoV-2 and to communicate this data more clearly to the general public. Though the ‘knowledge-deficit’ explanation of low vaccine uptake might not work for well-established vaccines [ 42 ], we have found evidence that this might be different for COVID-19.

We are not recommending a policy of mandatory vaccination in this paper, but merely investigating the attitudes of people towards it. A policy of mandatory vaccination would be an extreme solution to solve the potential problem of low vaccine uptake, and a lot may be said in favor of less extreme policies (as outlined in [ 15 ], for instance). Vaccination could also be made mandatory only for certain groups of people (e.g. nurses, physicians, physiotherapists, people working in confined spaces, people travelling on public transport etc.), or only after time has conclusively shown that not enough people actually get vaccinated. It might also turn out that people are unwilling to take the second dose of a two-dose vaccine, or not accept refresher doses, which would further complicate the situation and might require subtle intertemporal strategy choice. Before making any vaccination mandatory, people could also be paid or incentivized in other ways to accept it [ 43 ]. If, as we hope, people take the external effects of their action into account, and a sufficiently high number of people get vaccinated as a result, mandatory vaccination won’t be necessary.

This article investigates the willingness to get vaccinated and the acceptance of a policy of mandatory vaccination against COVID-19 in June and July 2020 in Germany. Our first main result is that a large majority of about 70 percent of adults in Germany would voluntarily get vaccinated against the novel coronavirus if a vaccine without side effects was available. Our second result is that about half of this population is in favor of, and half against, a policy of mandatory vaccination. Our third main result is that the individual willingness to get vaccinated and acceptance of a policy of mandatory vaccination correlates systematically with several socio-demographics (gender, age, education, income) but, overall, not with psychological characteristics of the respondents.

When interpreting the results from our survey, it should be noted that preferences were elicited in an ideal-typical situation: a vaccine which is effective and free of side effects is immediately available for the entire population at zero cost. Future research will have to show how actual vaccination behavior differs in real-life situations that deviate from this ideal-typical situation.

Supporting information

S1 file. variable definitions..

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248372.s001

S2 File. Comparison with further studies in Germany.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248372.s002

S3 File. Complementary estimation results.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248372.s003

S4 File. Additional diagnostics for the logit models.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248372.s004

S5 File. Multicollinearity across explanatory variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248372.s005

S6 File. Functional form assumptions and simultaneity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248372.s006

S7 File. Imputation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248372.s007

S8 File. Stata code.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248372.s008

Acknowledgments

We thank Thomas Rieger for his outstanding research assistance.

  • 1. WHO. Draft Landscape of COVID-19 candidate vaccines. 2020. Available from: https://www.who.int/publications/m/item/draft-landscape-of-COVID-19-candidate-vaccines (accessed: March 2, 2021).
  • View Article
  • Google Scholar
  • 5. Stiglitz JE. Economics of the public sector. 2nd edn. W.W. Norton & Co.: New York. 1988.
  • PubMed/NCBI
  • 17. Mill JS. On Liberty. In: Collected Works of John Stuart Mill , ed. Robson J.M. Toronto: University of Toronto Press, London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1963–1991). 1859; 33. https://oll.libertyfund.org/titles/165 .
  • 19. Giubilini A. The Ethics of Vaccination. Palgrave Studies in Ethics and Public Policy. Open Access. 2019. https://link.springer.com/book/10.1007%2F978-3-030-02068-2 .
  • 21. Schröder C, Goebel J, Grabka MM, Graeber D, Kröger H, Kroh M, et al. Vor dem COVID-19-Virus sind nicht alle Erwerbstätigen gleich. DIW aktuell. 2020; 41.
  • 23. Siegers R, Belcheva V, Silbermann T. SOEP-Core v35 Documentation of Sample Sizes and Panel Attrition in the German Socio-Economic Panel (SOEP) (1984 until 2018). SOEP Survey Papers. 2020; 826.
  • 24. Kroh M, Siegers R, Kühne S. Gewichtung und Integration von Auffrischungsstichproben am Beispiel des Sozio-oekonomischen Panels (SOEP), in Nonresponse Bias: Qualitätssicherung Sozialwissenschaftlicher Umfragen. Eds.: Schupp J. und Wolf . Wiesbaden: Springer Fachmedien Wiesbaden. 2015; C: 409–44.
  • 25. Siegers S, Steinhauer HW, Zinn S. Gewichtung der SOEP-CoV-Studie 2020, SOEP Survey Papers, Series C–Data Documentation. 2020; 888.
  • 28. Navin M. Values and Vaccine Refusal: Hard Questions in Epistemology, Ethics and Health Care, New York: Routledge. 2016.
  • Type 2 Diabetes
  • Heart Disease
  • Digestive Health
  • Multiple Sclerosis
  • COVID-19 Vaccines
  • Occupational Therapy
  • Healthy Aging
  • Health Insurance
  • Public Health
  • Patient Rights
  • Caregivers & Loved Ones
  • End of Life Concerns
  • Health News
  • Thyroid Test Analyzer
  • Doctor Discussion Guides
  • Hemoglobin A1c Test Analyzer
  • Lipid Test Analyzer
  • Complete Blood Count (CBC) Analyzer
  • What to Buy
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Medical Expert Board

An Overview of the Vaccine Debate

Looking at Both Sides of the Argument

There is a wealth of research demonstrating the efficacy and safety of vaccines —including how some have virtually eradicated infectious diseases that once killed millions. However, this has done little to sway those who believe that untold harms are being hidden from the American public.

The vaccine debate—including the argument as to whether vaccines are safe, effective, or could cause conditions like autism —has received a lot of attention from the media in recent years. With so much conflicting information being publicized, it can be a challenge to discern what is true and what is not. Therefore, it is important to learn the facts before making health decisions.

Claims and Controversy

Those who are part of the anti-vaccination movement include not only non-medical professionals but several scientists and healthcare providers who hold alternative views about vaccines and vaccination in general.

Some notable examples include:

  • British healthcare provider Andrew Wakefield, who in 1998 published research linking the MMR vaccine and autism . That study has since been retracted, and he was later removed from the medical registry in the United Kingdom for falsifying scientific data.
  • Pediatrician Bob Sears, who wrote the bestseller "The Vaccine Book: Making the Right Decision for your Child ," which suggested that many essential childhood vaccines were "optional." However, he was subsequently put on probation by the Medical Review Board of California in 2018 for alleged medical negligence and the inappropriate writing of medical exemptions for vaccinations.
  • Dr. Jane M. Orient, director of the Association of American Healthcare Providers and Surgeons, who was among the leading opponents of the COVID-19 vaccine and one of the leading proponents of using hydroxychloroquine to treat COVID-19 during the pandemic.

These opposing views and claims, along with other information promoted by the news and social media, have led some people to question whether they know everything they need to know about vaccines.

Common Concerns Regarding Vaccines

The arguments made against vaccines are not new and have been made well before the first vaccine was developed for smallpox back in the 18th century.

The following are some of the common arguments against vaccines:

  • Vaccines contain "toxic" ingredients that can lead to an assortment of chronic health conditions such as autism.
  • Vaccines are a tool of "Big Pharma," in which manufacturers are willing to profit off of harm to children.
  • Governments are "pharma shills," meaning they are bought off by pharmaceutical companies to hide cures or approve drugs that are not safe.
  • A child’s immune system is too immature to handle vaccines , leading the immune system to become overwhelmed and trigger an array of abnormal health conditions.
  • Natural immunity is best , suggesting that a natural infection that causes disease is "better" than receiving a vaccine that may cause mild side effects.
  • Vaccines are not tested properly , suggesting a (highly unethical) approach in which one group of people is given a vaccine, another group is not, and both are intentionally inoculated with the same virus or bacteria.
  • Infectious diseases have declined due in part to improved hygiene and sanitation , suggesting that hand-washing and other sanitary interventions are all that are needed to prevent epidemics.
  • Vaccines cause the body to "shed" virus , a claim that is medically true, although the amount of shed virus is rarely enough to cause infection.

The impact of anti-vaccination claims has been profound. For example, it has led to a resurgence of measles in the United States and Europe, despite the fact that the disease was declared eliminated in the U.S. back in 2000.

Studies have suggested that the anti-vaccination movement has cast doubt on the importance of childhood vaccinations among large sectors of the population. The added burden of the COVID-19 pandemic has led to further declines in vaccination rates.

There is also concern that the same repercussions may affect COVID-19 vaccination rates—both domestically and abroad. Ultimately, vaccine rates must be high for herd immunity to be effective.

According to a study from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), the rate of complete recommended vaccination among babies age 5 months has declined from 66.6% in 2016 to 49.7% by May 2020. Declines in vaccination coverage were seen in other age groups as well.

Benefits of Vaccination

Of the vaccines recommended by the CDC, the benefits of immunization are seen to overwhelmingly outweigh the potential risks. While there are some people who may need to avoid certain vaccines due to underlying health conditions, the vast majority can do so safely.

According to the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, there are five important reasons why your child should get the recommended vaccines:

  • Immunizations can save your child’s life . Consider that polio once killed up to 30% of those who developed paralytic symptoms. Due to polio vaccination, the disease is no longer a public health concern in the United States.
  • Vaccination is very safe and effective . Injection site pain and mild, flu-like symptoms may occur with vaccine shots. However, serious side effects , such as a severe allergic reaction, are very rare.
  • Immunization protects others . Because respiratory viruses can spread easily among children, getting your child vaccinated not only protects your child but prevents the further spread of disease.
  • Immunizations can save you time and money . According to the non-profit Borgen Project, the average cost of a measles vaccination around the world is roughly $1.76, whereas the average cost of treating measles is $307. In the end, the cost of prevention is invariably smaller than the cost of treatment.
  • Immunization protects future generations . Smallpox vaccinations have led to the eradication of smallpox . Rubella (German measles) vaccinations have helped eliminate birth defects caused by infection of pregnant mothers in the developed world. With persistence and increased community uptake, measles could one day be declared eliminated (again) as well.

A Word From Verywell

If you have any questions or concerns about vaccinations, do not hesitate to speak with your healthcare provider or your child's pediatrician.

If a vaccine on the immunization schedule has been missed, speak to a healthcare provider before seeking the vaccination on your own (such as at a pharmacy or clinic). In some cases, additional doses may be needed.

Vaccines Healthcare Provider Discussion Guide

Get our printable guide for your next healthcare provider's appointment to help you ask the right questions.

Sign up for our Health Tip of the Day newsletter, and receive daily tips that will help you live your healthiest life.

Thank you, {{form.email}}, for signing up.

There was an error. Please try again.

Eggerton L.  Lancet retracts 12-year-old article linking autism to MMR vaccines .  CMAJ . 2010 Mar 9; 182(4):e199-200. doi:10.1503/cmaj.109-3179

Park A. Doctor behind vaccine-autism link loses license . Time .

Offit PA, Moser CA.  The problem with Dr Bob's alternative vaccine schedule .  Pediatrics.  2009 Jan;123 (1):e164-e169. doi:10.1542/peds.2008-2189

Before the Medical Board of California, Department of Consumer Affairs, State of California. In the Matter of the Accusation Against Robert William Sears, M.D., Case No. 800-2015-012268 .

Stolberg SG. Anti-vaccine doctor has been invited to testify before Senate committee . The New York Times.

Wolfe RM, Sharp LK.  Anti-vaccinationists past and present . BMJ. 2002;325(7361):430-2. doi:10.1136/bmj.325.7361.430

Agley J, Xiao Y. Misinformation about COVID-19: Evidence for differential latent profiles and a strong association with trust in science . BMC Public Health. 2021;21:89. doi:10.1186/s12889-020-10103-x

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Measles history .

Hussain A, Ali S, Ahmed M, Hussain S. The anti-vaccination movement: a regression in modern medicine .  Cureus . 2018;10(7): e2919. doi:10.7759/cureus.2919

Bramer CA, Kimmins LM, Swanson R, et al. Decline in child vaccination coverage during the COVID-19 pandemic — Michigan Care Improvement Registry, May 2016–May 2020 . MMWR. 2020 May;69(20):630-1. doi:10.15585/mmwr.mm6920e1

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Why vaccinate .

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Poliomyelitis .

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Making the vaccine decision .

Borgen Project. What is the cost of measles in the developed world? .

By Vincent Iannelli, MD  Vincent Iannelli, MD, is a board-certified pediatrician and fellow of the American Academy of Pediatrics. Dr. Iannelli has cared for children for more than 20 years. 

  • Open access
  • Published: 09 November 2023

To vaccinate or not to vaccinate? The interplay between pro- and against- vaccination reasons

  • Marta Caserotti 1 ,
  • Paolo Girardi 2 ,
  • Roberta Sellaro 1 ,
  • Enrico Rubaltelli 1 ,
  • Alessandra Tasso 3 ,
  • Lorella Lotto 1 &
  • Teresa Gavaruzzi 4  

BMC Public Health volume  23 , Article number:  2207 ( 2023 ) Cite this article

1919 Accesses

1 Altmetric

Metrics details

By mid 2023, European countries reached 75% of vaccine coverage for COVID-19 and although vaccination rates are quite high, many people are still hesitant. A plethora of studies have investigated factors associated with COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy, however, insufficient attention has been paid to the reasons why people get vaccinated against COVID-19. Our work aims to investigate the role of reasons in the decision to get vaccinated against COVID-19 in a representative sample of 1,689 adult Italians (March–April 2021) balanced in terms of age, gender, educational level and area of residence.

Through an online questionnaire, we asked participants to freely report up to three reasons for and against COVID-19 vaccination, and the weight each had in the decision to get vaccinated. We first investigated the role of emotional competence and COVID-19 risk perception in the generation of both reasons using regression models. Next, we studied the role that the different reasons had in the vaccination decision, considering both the intention to vaccinate (using a beta regression model) and the decision made by the participants who already had the opportunity to get vaccinated (using a logistic regression model). Finally, two different classification tree analyses were carried out to characterize profiles with a low or high willingness to get vaccinated or with a low or high probability to accept/book the vaccine.

High emotional competence positively influences the generation of both reasons (ORs > 1.5), whereas high risk perception increases the generation of positive reasons (ORs > 1.4) while decreasing reasons against vaccination (OR = 0.64). As pro-reasons increase, vaccination acceptance increases, while the opposite happens as against-reasons increase (all p  < 0.001). One strong reason in favor of vaccines is enough to unbalance the decision toward acceptance of vaccination, even when reasons against it are also present ( p  < 0.001). Protection and absence of distrust are the reasons that mostly drive willingness to be vaccinated and acceptance of an offered vaccine.

Conclusions

Knowing the reasons that drive people’s decision about such an important choice can suggest new communication insights to reduce possible negative reactions toward vaccination and people's hesitancy. Results are discussed considering results of other national and international studies.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

By mid 2023 the European Union reached nearly 75% vaccine coverage for the primary vaccine cycle against COVID-19, with countries such as Croatia, Slovakia, and Poland falling short of 60% and others such as France, Portugal, and Italy close to 90% [ 1 ]. Although vaccination rates are, on average, quite high, many people are still hesitant. Vaccine hesitancy indicates the delay or refusal of a vaccine despite availability in vaccine services [ 2 , 3 ] and is a multidimensional construct, resulting from the interaction between individual, social, and community aspects [ 4 ]. In the last two years, a plethora of studies have investigated factors associated with COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy showing, for example, that vaccine hesitancy is higher in women [ 5 , 6 ], in young people [ 5 , 7 , 8 ], in people with low education [ 8 , 9 ], low trust in authorities [ 10 , 11 ], and strong conspiracy beliefs [ 5 , 12 , 13 ]. However, to the best of our knowledge no one has investigated the interplay that pro- and against- vaccination reasons may play in the choice to get vaccinated, namely what happens when a person has both pro- and against-vaccine considerations. Trying to fill this gap in the literature, our work aims to investigate how different reasons and the importance people place on them are likely to influence the decision to get vaccinated against COVID-19.

In line with the vaccine hesitancy continuum defined by SAGE [ 2 ], while extremely pro-vax people are likely to express only reasons pro-vaccination and extremely no-vax people are likely to express only reasons against vaccination, individuals who fall between the two extreme end-points are likely to feel some doubts. This large number of people offer us the unique opportunity to assess which category of reasons (pro- vs. against- vaccination) is more impactful in driving people's vaccination decisions. As it is reasonable to imagine, among the reasons for choosing to get (or not) vaccinated some reasons are more rational, while others are more related to affect. For example, there are people who rationally recognize the importance of vaccines but at the same time are frightened by the side effects. Thus, the decision to get (or not) vaccinated is the result of a complex process, in which costs and benefits are weighed more or less rationally. Indeed, while several studies have pointed out that the decision to vaccinate is due to cognitive rather than emotional processes [ 14 , 15 , 16 , 17 ], others have highlighted the role of affect and risk perception in the vaccination decision [ 18 , 19 , 20 ]. Thus, the intention to accept the vaccine is driven by emotional and affective feelings as much as by cognitive and rational judgments. Particular attention to what people feel and think about vaccine-preventable diseases and vaccination in general is paid in the model developed by the “Measuring Behavioral and Social Drivers of Vaccination” (BeSD), a global group of experts established by the World Health Organization [ 21 ]. This model encompasses two groups of proximal antecedents of vaccination, namely, what people think and feel (e.g., perceived risk, worry, confidence, trust and safety concerns) and social processes (e.g., provider recommendation, social norms and rumors). Antecedents affect vaccination motivation (i.e., vaccination readiness, willingness, intention, hesitancy), which can then be strengthened or weakened by practical issues (such as vaccine availability, convenience and cost but also requirements and incentives), resulting in acceptance, delay or refusal of vaccination (vaccination behavior).

Although some studies have considered whether the cognitive or affective component has greater weight in determining the intention to vaccinate, no one, to the best of our knowledge, has studied the interplay between pro- and against- vaccination reasons, nor the weight these have in the choice to vaccinate. In addition to the drivers already studied in the literature [ 5 , 6 , 7 , 8 , 11 , 12 ], we believe that the focus on this interaction may be relevant to better understand the complex phenomena related to vaccine hesitancy. Few recent studies have attempted to investigate the complexity of vaccination choice by studying the reasons why people choose to get (or not) vaccinated against COVID-19. Fieselmann and colleagues [ 22 ] highlighted that among the reasons that reduce adherence to vaccination are a low perception of its benefits, a low perception of the risk of contracting COVID-19, health concerns, lack of information, distrust of the system, and spiritual or religious reasons. Another study, instead, shed light on the reasons that encourage hesitant people to consider vaccination, such as protecting themselves, their family, friends and community from COVID-19, and being able to return to normal life [ 23 ].

In the present study we asked the participants to spontaneously come up with their own reasons to get (or not) vaccinated, without limiting or influencing them with a set of predefined options to choose from, thus aiming to obtain more genuine answers that may better capture the intuitive aspect of people’s opinions (for a similar reasoning see [ 24 ]). The procedure we used has been implemented by Moore et al. [ 23 ], the only study, as far as we know, that asked for reasons with an open-ended question. Critically, in their study, participants were asked to report only reasons in favor of vaccination (e.g., "What are your reasons for getting the COVID-19 vaccine?"), excluding reasons against. By contrast, we asked participants to freely report up to three reasons in favor and up to three reasons against COVID-19 vaccination and to rate on a 5-point Likert scale their weight in the decision about getting (or not) vaccinated.

From a theoretical point of view, the reasons pro- and against vaccination may be seen within the framework of prospect theory [ 25 , 26 ] which suggests that people evaluate the outcome of a choice based on a reference point, against which losses and gains are determined: the former below this point, the latter above this point. Importantly, especially in this specific context, losses and negative consequences are weighted more than gains and benefits, making us hypothesize that if a person has one reason for and one reason against the vaccine, which are of equal importance, they will more likely lean toward choosing not to vaccinate. Consistently, it is known that negative experiences have a greater impact than neutral or positive ones (i.e., the negativity bias [ 27 ]).

Besides delving into the reasons that may influence the choice to get (or not) vaccinated, it would be interesting to also look at the individual differences that may determine the reporting of pro- and against- vaccination reasons and their valence. In this regard, the literature suggests that risk perception and emotion regulation can both have a great impact in the decision to get vaccinated. For instance, studies conducted during H1N1 influenza have shown that perception of disease-related risk is one of the strongest predictors of vaccine adherence [ 28 , 29 ]. Additional insights have been provided by more recent studies investigating the role of COVID-19 risk perception in the decision to get vaccinated against COVID-19. Viswanath and colleagues [ 30 ] showed that people are more willing to vaccinate themselves and those under their care to the extent to which they feel more vulnerable to COVID-19 and rate the consequences of a possible infection as severe. Such a relationship between COVID-19 risk perception and intention to vaccinate was confirmed by another study using a cross-sectional design, which focused on the early months of the pandemic [ 31 ]. This study also examined how risk perception changed during the pandemic phases and showed that during the lockdown, compared to the pre-lockdown phase, also those who reported some hesitancy were more likely to get vaccinated when they perceived a strong COVID-19 risk.

With regard to emotion regulation, the literature suggests that people react differently to affective stimuli [ 32 ] and that their decisions are influenced by their abilities to regulate emotions [ 33 , 34 ]. Recent works investigating the relationship between hesitancy in pediatric vaccinations and the emotional load associated with vaccinations, have shown that a negative affective reaction is one of the factors leading to lower vaccine uptake [ 35 , 36 ]. Specifically, Gavaruzzi and colleagues [ 36 ] showed that concerns about vaccine safety and extreme views against vaccines are associated with vaccine refusal. Interestingly, they also showed that parents' intrapersonal emotional competences, i.e., their ability to manage, identify, and recognize their own emotions, is critical to vaccine acceptance for their children. Therefore, in our study we measured people's risk perception and emotional competencies to assess their possible role in the production of reasons in favor and against vaccination.

As described in Fig.  1 , the relationship between different domains of interest can be hierarchically structured, using a directed acyclic graph, starting from the risk perception and emotion regulation, to the generation of pro- and against- vaccination reasons and their valence, and finally to the vaccination willingness/adherence. With respect to the mentioned structure, we are interested to investigate the following research hypotheses:

The number and weight associated with reasons pro- and against-vaccination should be influenced by individual differences in the ability to regulate emotions;

The number and weight associated with pro-vaccination reasons should be influenced by individual differences in COVID-19 risk perception;

A higher number of strong (i.e., with high weight) reasons pro- (vs. against-) vaccination should correspond to a more (vs. less) likelihood to accept the vaccination.

Generating an equal number of reasons in favor and against vaccination should lead to a weaker likelihood to accept the vaccination.

figure 1

Directed Acyclic Graph (DAG) between variables considered in the study (PEC: Short Profile of Emotional Competence scale)

As we conducted the study between March and April 2021, a time when vaccinations were being progressively rolled out, we decided to consider the role of personal reasons on both the intention to get vaccinated (for those who had not yet had the opportunity to get vaccinated) and the choice already made (e.g., vaccine received or booked vs. refused).

Finally, through a non-parametric classification analysis, we will explore how specific pro- and against-vaccination reasons impact the decision to get (or not) vaccinated. Specifically, we will investigate the role that different categories of reasons play in the choice to vaccinate.

Participants

Data collection was commissioned to a survey and market research agency (Demetra Opinions.net), with the aim of securing a representative sample of the adult (+ 18) Italian population, estimated at 49.8 million [ 37 ]. The sample was balanced in terms of age, gender, educational level (middle school or lower, high school, degree or higher), and area of residence (North, Center, South, and Islands). The agency distributed via email the survey link to its panelists, who freely decided whether to participate in the study in exchange for financial compensation. Out of 1,833 participants who started the questionnaire, 77 (4%) were excluded because they did not complete the survey and 16 (0.9%) were excluded since they reported offensive content in open-ended questions. Finally, 124 (6.8%) participants were excluded because of missing information. Thus, the final sample consisted of 1,689 participants. The project was approved by the ethical committee for Psychology Research of the University of Padova (Italy), with protocol number 3911/2020 and informed consent was obtained for all participants.

We developed an ad-hoc questionnaire including a series of open-ended and closed questions (see Additional file 1 : Appendix 2 for the full material). We first investigated the vaccination status of the participants, asking whether they already had received at least the first dose, whether they had booked it or were still ineligible, and finally whether they had refused the vaccination. Those not yet eligible were asked to rate how likely they would be to get vaccinated at the time they responded (0 =  Not at all likely , 100 =  Extremely likely ). Then, we asked participants to report a maximum of three reasons both in favor of the COVID-19 vaccine and against it (in counterbalanced order) and to rate how much each of the reported reasons weighed in their choice to vaccinate or not, on a 5-point likert scale (1 =  Not at all , 5 =  Extremely ). Due to the sparsity on the rate and the number of provided reasons we re-coded the provided information into two semi-quantitative variables, one for pro- and one for against- vaccination reasons, as following: missing/invalid reasons, low average rating (answers 1–3 on the Likert scale) and 1–3 reasons, high rating (answers 4–5 points on the Likert scale) and 1 reason, and high average rating (answer 4–5 points on the Likert scale) and 2–3 reasons.

The questionnaire also included the 20-item Short Profile of Emotional Competence scale (S-PEC; [ 38 ]) to measure intra- and inter-personal emotional competences separately. The intra-personal scale (10 items) refers to emotional competences related to oneself and it includes items such as "In my life I never make decisions based on my emotions'' or "I don't always understand why I react in a certain way". The inter-personal scale (10 items) refers to emotional competences related to other people and it includes items such as “If I wanted, I could easily make someone feel uneasy” or “Most of the time, I understand why the people feel the way they do”. All items are answered on a 7-point likert scale (1 =  Not at all agree , 7 =  Completely agree ). The internal consistency of the S-PEC scale, measured by means of Cronbach’s α, was adequate (α = 0.81). Further, we measured participants' risk perception of COVID-19 by asking them to indicate how scared they felt of the virus, how serious they think the disease is, how likely they think they are to get sick, and how worried they feel about the various mutations [ 10 , 31 ]. We then asked participants to report their age, gender, educational level, their occupation (health workers, white-collar workers, entrepreneurs, other non-health-related contract forms, and the unemployed), whether they already had COVID-19 (No or don't know, Yes asymptomatic, Yes with few symptoms, and Yes with severe symptoms). The questionnaire was pilot tested by 30 participants who filled the questionnaire first then were asked to discuss and comment on the comprehension of the wording of questions and answer options. Two questions were slightly reworded to improve clarity.

Scoring of reasons

In the first instance, a bottom-up process from reasons to categories was followed by reading a sample of both types of reasons, with the aim of constructing initial categorizing patterns. Examples of pro-vaccination reasons include protection of personal and public health, return to normality, and civic duty; while reasons against vaccination include fears for one's health, sociopolitical perplexity, and distrust of science and institutions (see Additional file 1 : Appendix 1). At this stage, response information was added to the categorizations indicating whether the responses were valid or missing/invalid. Specifically, valid responses had both a reason and the respective weight; missing/invalid responses were those where reason, weight or both were missing or with utterly unrelated concepts or meaningless strings or letters. Finally, by applying a top-down process, we constructed macro categories by merging specific conceptually assimilated categories, so as to avoid the dispersion of data into too many ramifications (see Table S 5 ).

Statistical analysis

Descriptive analysis.

All the analyses were performed only on respondents with no missing observations on the variables of interest (1,681, 92%) excluding also a limited number of those with a non-valid set of pro- or against-vaccination reasons (Table S 1 ; 0.9%). The study variables were summarized in frequency tables and figures (frequency for categorical variables, median and Interquartile Range (IQR) for continuous variables). Kruskal–Wallis tests were computed to compare the distribution of continuous variables across the categories of vaccine status. Categorical variables were compared using chi-squared or Fisher's exact test where expected frequencies in any combination were less than 10. Statistical significance was assumed at the 5% level.

COVID-19 Perceived risk—exploratory factor analysis

An Exploratory Factorial Analysis (EFA) was performed on groups of variables related to COVID-19 perceived risk: scare, severity, contagiousness, and the likelihood of mutation. Since the presence of limited support (0–100 scale) and non-normal marginal distribution, the EFA was performed using a weighted least square mean and variance adjusted (WLSMV) estimator. We extracted from the EFA only the first factor, which explained the highest percentage of variance (Table S 2 ; 61%). The estimated loadings were then used to calculate the regression factor scores. The number and the name of items included, their internal consistency (Cronbach’s α), the estimated loadings, and the proportion of deviance explained are reported in Table S 2 .

Propensity score weighting

At the time of data collection (March–April 2021), the vaccine offer was not opened to the entire population. To adjust the estimates of the following regression models for the propensity to receive the vaccine, we estimated a logistic regression model in which the dependent variable was the response to the question about a previous vaccination offer (Yes/No), while all the factors that can influence the vaccine proposal served as independent variables: age-class (young ≤ 25, young adult 26–45, adult 46–65, elderly 66–84), gender (male, female), occupational status (health worker, not at work, not health worker-employer, not health worker-entrepreneur, not health worker-other), educational level (low = middle school or lower, medium = high school, high = degree or higher), key worker status (yes, no, I don’t know), past COVID-19 contagion (no, yes asymptomatic, yes low symptoms, yes severe symptoms), and familiar status (single/in a relation, married/cohabitant, divorced/separated/other). The predicted probability was used to estimate the weights for the following regression models using a framework based on an inverse probability of treatment weighting (IPTW; for further details, see [ 39 ]).

Regression models

Our research questions can be summarized by trying to describe the relationship exploited by the directed acyclic graph in Fig.  1 . The first step regression model aims to assess how S-PEC scores (inter- and intra-personal) and COVID-19 risk perception influenced the reasons pro- and against-vaccination produced by participants while considering the presence of a set of confounders (age-class, gender, occupational status, educational level, key worker status, and familial status).

Since both the pro- and against-vaccination reasons are formed by a categorical variable with 4 levels (missing/invalid, low 1/2/3 reasons, high 1 reason, high 2/3 reasons), we evaluated whether S-PEC and COVID-19 risk perception scores influenced the distribution of pro- and against-vaccination reasons employing two different multinomial regression models including all the previously mentioned variables (S-PEC, COVID-19 risk perception, and confounders). The overall significance of a variable in the model was tested using an analysis of the variance (ANOVA).

The second step in the analyses was taken to investigate whether the generation of pro- and/or against-vaccination reasons affected the willingness to be vaccinated or the vaccine acceptance. Each participant reported their willingness to get vaccinated on a 0–100 scale or, in case a COVID-19 vaccine had been already offered, their vaccination status (done, booked, or refused). For respondents who had not yet been contacted for booking/getting the vaccination, we evaluated whether pro- and/or against vaccination reasons influenced the willingness to be vaccinated by employing a beta regression model in which the respondent variable scale (0–100) was rescaled to be a relative frequency [ 40 ]. The full models included the semi-quantitative pro- and against-vaccination reasons variables and, even if non-statistically significant, all the confounders in order to adjust for age class, gender, educational level, occupational status, familial status, and key worker status. Beta regression coefficients were estimated using a maximum likelihood estimator (MLE). Results were presented in terms of Odds Ratios (ORs) by exponentiating the estimated coefficients and producing a relative 95% Confidence Interval (95% CI).

A further regression analysis was conducted through a logistic regression model to explain which factors influenced vaccine acceptance (done/booked vs. refused) among those who already received the vaccine offers. The full model included the same variables considered in the previous beta regression model, after recoding the variables related to pro- and against-vaccination reasons into a binary form (missing/invalid vs. presence of at least one valid reason) due to low sample size and the sparsity of the response variable. As a consequence, we tested a simplified version of Hypothesis 3, considering the presence (vs. missing/invalid) of pro- or against-vaccination reasons in order to test their influence on the probability of having accepted/booked the vaccination.

Results were reported employing ORs and relative 95% Confidence Interval (95% CI).

Both the beta regression and logistic regression were weighed using an IPTW scheme to take into account the presence of a different probability of a vaccine offer among respondents.

The presence of an interaction between pro- and against-vaccination reasons was tested by means of a likelihood ratio test. The regression models were estimated through the R 4.0 program (R Core Team, 2021), and for the beta regression we employed the betareg package [ 41 ].

Classification tree analysis

Two different classification tree analyses were carried out to characterize profiles with a low or high willingness to get vaccinated (respondents who had not yet been offered a vaccine) or with a low or high probability to accept/book the vaccine (respondents who had already received a vaccine offer).

Although the dependent variables were non-normally distributed (scale 0–100 or binary 0/1), we considered them continuously distributed adopting a splitting criterion based on the analysis of the variance (ANOVA). We tested the inclusion in the model considering the type of pro- or against-vaccination reasons. A tree pruning strategy was adopted to reduce classification tree overfitting considering the overall determination coefficient (R 2 ) as an indicator and fixing that at each classification step in the tree if the R 2 did not increase by 0.5% the tree should be stopped. Classification tree analysis was performed using the rpart package [ 42 ] on R environment [ 43 ].

The main characteristics of the respondents by vaccination status (received, booked, not yet, and refused) were reported in Table 1 . Among respondents, 23.3% were offered the vaccination and, among them, 13.8% refused it (Fig. S 1 ). Among those not yet eligible, willingness to be vaccinated showed a median value of 80 points (average: 68.7). The distribution of gender was almost equal (51% females, 49% male), and the median age was 47 years old (IQR: 34–57 years). Educational level was low in 41% of the sample, while the most represented employment status was not at work (39%) followed by employed (37%), and entrepreneur (9.8%). A quarter (26%) of respondents classified themselves as key workers during the COVID-19 pandemic. The predominance of respondents (63%) were married or living with a partner, while only 9% had had a COVID-19 infection.

COVID-19 risk perception and the S-PEC score (intra- and inter-personal) were categorized into three categories according to empirical tertiles (low:1 st tertile, medium: 2 nd tertile, high: 3 rd tertile). The level of COVID-19 risk perception differed across vaccination status ( p  < 0.001). The reasons pro- and against-vaccination have a different distribution according to COVID-19 vaccination status (Table 2 ). The highest frequency of pro-vaccination reasons was reported by those who received the COVID-19 vaccination; conversely the lowest frequency of pro-vaccination reasons was generated by those who refused the vaccine, whereas, intermediate frequencies were shown by people who were not yet offered the vaccination and those who had booked the vaccine, who reported a comparable distribution of the number of pro-vaccination reasons. A reverse pattern was exhibited for against-vaccination reasons, which were generated with the highest percentage by respondents who refused the vaccine (in particular high and multiple reasons). Conversely those who have booked/done the COVID-19 vaccine showed the lowest frequency of reasons against vaccination, while respondents without a vaccine offer reported an intermediate frequency of reasons against vaccination.

The estimated results of the propensity score model for the vaccine offer are shown in Table S 3 . Respondents older than 65 years exhibited a nearly four-fold increase in the probability to be contacted for the vaccination with respect to the reference age-class (≤ 25 years). All non-health employees showed a high drop in the probability of having received the vaccination offer, while the probability increased as the educational level increased. Being a key worker during pandemic resulted in an increased probability of having received the vaccination proposal while no statistical significant influence was observed for the past COVID-19 contagion or for familial status. The distribution of the propensity score by vaccine status obtained by the model is reported in Fig. S 1 , in which it is shown that the distribution is different by vaccine offer, but the two density functions partially overlap. The discriminant power of the propensity score estimated was only discrete (ROC analysis, AUC: 71.8%).

The results of the multinomial regression models which investigated the effect of emotional competences and risk perception on the generation and the predictors of pro- and against-vaccination reasons with respect to missing/invalid level and the reference categories are presented in Table 3 (see also Fig.  1 ). Compared to the reference category (missing/invalid), high values of S-PEC-self were associated with a higher probability to report pro- and against-vaccination reasons (all ORs > 1.5), while high values of S-PEC-others were associated with a mild probability to report multiple pro-vaccination reasons (all ORs > 1.42). A high (vs. low) COVID-19 risk perception increased the frequency of one strong pro-vaccination reason while it had a null or low decremental effect on the frequency of against weak vaccination reasons. Further, medium (vs. low) COVID-19 risk perception only increased the strong pro-vaccination. Compared to the reference age-class (young), adults and elderly showed a higher probability to generate a strong unique pro-vaccination reason (adults vs. young OR: 1.72, 95%CI: 1.07–2.77); elderly vs. young OR: 2.24, 95%CI: 1.26–4.00), while lower probability to generate against vaccination reasons was observed for elderly compared to young respondents (OR: 0.48, 95%CI: 0.26–0.90). Female participants generated fewer strong pro-vaccination reasons (ORs < 0.73), and also fewer multiple weak against-vaccination reasons (OR: 0.68, 95%CI: 0.51–0.91) compared to male participants. Overall, the occupational status did not affect the generation of pro- and against-vaccination reasons (ANOVA test p  > 0.05); however an increased frequency of low 1/2/3 against-vaccination reasons emerged among the category “Other—not health workers” compared to the reference group represented by health workers (OR: 2.52, 95%CI:1.09–5.86). Pro-vaccination reasons are more frequent as the educational level becomes higher, while the relation of the educational level with against- vaccination reasons appears weaker and significantly increased only for the presence of multiple weak reasons against vaccination (High vs. Low educational level, OR: 2.10, 95%CI: 1.45–3.03). Not being a key worker is related to a higher frequency of multiple strong both pro- and against vaccination reasons. The familiar status did not seem to be related to the frequency or the strength of the reasons, except for the status of divorced/separate/other that, with respect to the reference category single/in a relation, showed a twofold increase in the frequency of a strong unique against vaccination reason.

Through a beta regression model we investigated the predictors of willingness to be vaccinated for the participants who had not yet received the vaccination offer. As shown in Table 4 , the generation of pro- and against-vaccination reasons strongly influences the willingness to be vaccinated. The predicted probability from the combination of pro- and against-vaccination reasons is shown in Fig.  2 (and Table S 4 ): respondents who did not report any reasons had an average predicted probability above 60%, while the presence of at least one reason against vaccination decreased the willingness to be vaccinated, in particular in the case of strong multiple against vaccination reasons. On the other hand, the presence of at least one pro-vaccination reason strongly increased the probability. In the end, the presence of both strong multiple pro and against vaccination reasons resulted in a high probability of getting the vaccine. Regression models adjusted by propensity score weighting allowed us to comment the influence of potential confounders: males reported an increased willingness to be vaccinated (vs. females; OR: 1.26, 95%CI: 1.11–1.42), and so did those with a high educational level (vs. low; OR: 1.22, 95%CI: 1.04–1.44) while the opposite was true among no key workers (vs. key workers; OR: 0.85, 95%CI: 0.72–0.99).

figure 2

Predicted willingness to get vaccinated by interaction between pro- and against-vaccination reasons

Finally, with a logistic model we investigated the predictors of vaccine acceptance\booking. As shown in Table 5 , people who accepted or booked the COVID-19 vaccine were more likely to show pro-vaccination reasons and less likely to show against-vaccination reasons. Interestingly, when both kinds of reasons were provided, the probability of getting/booking the vaccine remained nevertheless very high (Fig.  3 ). Compared to the age class [46-65], younger age classes reported a strong reduction in the probability to have accepted/booked the vaccine. Male participants (OR: 1.53, 95%CI: 1.10–2.12) and those with a high educational level (OR: 2.65, 95%CI: 1.60–4.54) showed an increased probability of vaccine acceptance/booking when compared to females and participants with medium educational level, respectively. Being a health worker had a strong and positive influence on the probability of getting/booking the vaccine with respect to those employed as no health workers (OR: 6.61, 95%CI: 2.10–30.9).

figure 3

Predicted COVID-19 vaccine acceptance/booking probability by interaction between pro- and against-vaccination reasons

Two regression tree models were estimated separately on the willingness to be vaccinated for those who had not yet received the vaccine offer and on the booking/acceptance of the vaccination in case of vaccine offer. Results are shown in Fig.  4 . Considering the willingness to be vaccinated, the presence of distrust in the vaccination was the most discriminant variable; this latter in conjunction with reasons related to protection, herd immunity, and the absence of no clinical trials guided the willingness to be vaccinated. In particular, the combination of the absence of reasons related to distrust and the presence of protection reasons showed the highest values on the intention to get vaccinated (average value = 83 points, 22% of the sample). On the other side, the presence of at least one reason related to distrust without any positive reasons concerning protection, herd immunity, and trust predicted the lowest willingness to be vaccinated (average value = 29 points, 6% of the sample).

figure 4

Regression tree for the willingness to be vaccinated (left) and for COVID-19 vaccine acceptance/booking (right) by selected type of pro- and against-vaccination reasons

The sense of protection given by the vaccine or the trust in the vaccination was the main reason for vaccination acceptance/booking (average probability = 0.96 and 1.00, 33% and 5% of the sample, respectively). The combination of the absence of protective reasons and the presence of doubts about the lack of clinical studies results in the lowest likelihood of accepting/booking the vaccination (average probability = 0.40, 3% of the sample). The presence of distrust and the belief in herd immunity were the other discriminant reasons with intermediate results in terms of the probability to accept/book the vaccination.

The frequency of each category of pro- and against-vaccination reasons by COVID-19 vaccine status is shown in Table S 5 .

In the present study we aimed to investigate the reasons behind the decision to get (or not) vaccinated against COVID-19 by asking participants to report up to three reasons in favor and three reasons against the COVID-19 vaccination and to indicate the weight these reasons had in their decision. Although some researchers discourage categorization, the sparsity of the responses related to the number of reasons and their weight implies a semi-quantitative solution since a simple variable multiplication between rating and frequency (recoding to zero in case of zero reasons) is not feasible. In this case, this approach was not satisfactory as such coding would not allow differences underlying identical scores to emerge. For example, only 1 strong motivation (rating 5) would be coded in the same way as three motivations with weights 1, 2, and 2. Instead, we decided to categorize the combination of frequency-weight reasons as categorical variables (missing/invalid, low 1/2/3 reasons, high 1 reason, high 2/3 reasons) in which rating and number of reasons are combined into a single variable. This categorization allows us not only to study the weight that different categories have on the decision to get vaccinated but also to overcome the risk of imputing a specific value for missing responses.

As shown in Fig.  1 , analyses were run in two steps. The first step aimed to assess how emotional competences and risk perception impacted the generation of reasons pro- and against-vaccination (Hypotheses 1A and 1B), whereas the second step investigated how different reasons affected the intention to get vaccinated (Hypotheses 2 and 3). The results support the hypotheses that emotional competences and risk perception play a significant role. Regarding emotional competence as measured by the S-PEC, the results show that high intra-personal emotional competence positively influences the production of stronger and more numerous pro-vaccination and against-vaccination reasons (confirming Hypothesis 1A). This result suggests that greater awareness of one's emotions and of what one is feeling promotes higher fluency in the production of reasons about the vaccination. Research has shown that people can be ambivalent about vaccines and hold both positive and negative reasons [ 2 , 44 ]. It is reasonable to assume that, compared to people with low intra-personal emotional competences, those with high intra-personal emotional competences are more likely to have higher awareness of these contrasting attitudes and to embrace them without suppressing one of the two stances. Furthermore, the results showed that only high inter-personal emotional competences influence the generation of multiple strong reasons in favor of vaccination, and this appears to be related to the perception of vaccines as a public good and a tool to protect others. As for risk perception, a moderate to high perception of risk associated with COVID-19 influences the generation of strong pro-vaccination reasons (confirming Hypothesis 1B). These results are in line with the literature showing that a high perception of risk associated with COVID-19 positively influences the decision to get vaccinated [ 30 , 31 , 45 , 46 , 47 ]. In particular, perceiving a medium/high risk leads to generating a high number of reasons strongly in favor of vaccination, while reducing the number and weight of the reasons against the vaccine. The main premise of the psychological literature examining the relationship between risk perception and affect is that one’s behaviors are affected by rapid and intuitive evaluations, either positive or negative, people make while assessing things happening around them [ 48 , 49 ]. Thus, an event is evaluated not only on the basis of objective information, but also on the basis of the experienced feelings. Emotional competence, which is clearly related to affect, also modulates how we perceive and process the emotional component underlying our judgments [ 36 ].

The results also show that, compared with younger people, those over 45 more frequently produce reasons in favor of vaccines while those over 65 produce fewer reasons against vaccination. These results are in line with the fact that younger people are at lower risk of severe consequences than older people [ 50 ], but can also be explained by considering that age was particularly salient during the period of the data collection, as the vaccination campaign was phased out by age groups, starting from the elderly. As for gender, women produced less strong pro-vaccine and weak-against vaccine reasons than men. These results are congruent with the general findings in the literature on vaccine hesitancy showing that females are more hesitant than males [ 5 , 51 , 52 ]. Furthermore, medium and high educational levels favored the production of both pro- and against-vaccination reasons, whereas not being in a relationship or being divorced/separated increased the generation of a strong reason against vaccination. Consistent with previous work [ 53 ], we confirmed that non-health professionals participants or non-key workers categories showed a lower intention to get vaccinated and a higher likelihood of having refused the vaccine compared to health care and key workers.

Once the role of demographics aspects and individual differences on the generation of reasons pro and/or against vaccination had been established, we ran two additional models to assess the role that those reasons have on the decision to accept the vaccination (see Fig.  1 ). More specifically, we tested the hypothesis that a higher number of pro- (vs. against-) vaccination reasons, connoted by a higher weight, corresponded to a stronger (vs. weaker) acceptance of vaccination (Hypothesis 2). Since data collection took place between March and April 2021, when the vaccination campaign had already started in Italy, we developed two different regression models, with the first investigating the willingness to be vaccinated in participants who were not yet offered the vaccine and the second investigating the likelihood of accepting/booking or refusing the vaccine in those who already received the offer. In particular, thanks to the propensity score weighting technique, we managed to reduce the estimates bias, especially for those factors (age, occupational status, and educational level) that influenced the vaccine offer the most [ 54 ]. The results of the two models are very similar, as the intention to get vaccinated and the likelihood of having accepted/booked the vaccine are predicted by the same factors. Specifically, the production of strong positive reasons increases either the intention to get vaccinated or having accepted/booked the vaccination. In contrast, generating strong negative reasons reduces vaccination intention and predicts the refusal of the vaccination. Hypothesis 2 is thus confirmed.

Results on the interactions between reasons, pro- and against-vaccination, and vaccination intention or vaccination choice are particularly worthy of attention. The third hypothesis was derived from the literature on prospect theory [ 25 , 26 ], suggesting that at equal intensity subjective losses are more important in determining a decision than subjective gains. We therefore expected that negative reasons would count more than positive reasons in deciding whether to get vaccinated or to accept the vaccine. However, in contrast to our hypothesis, the results showed that just the generation of a single positive reason with a strong weight was enough to shift behavior and attitude in favor of the vaccination, regardless of the number and weight of negative reasons. In other words, vaccine refusal is predicted by the absence of any positive strong reasons, while when people generate both positive and negative reasons, the positive ones seem to yield a particularly important role when having a strong weight. According to prospect theory, people evaluate their goals depending on the reference point they focus on. During the pandemic, the vaccination offered an opportunity to be safer, reduced the risk of infection, and more generally appeared as the best way to re-open and get back to life as it was before COVID-19. After a year of pandemic characterized by periods of lockdown and some re-opening attempts, people were likely feeling in a state of loss (e.g., the lost freedom to go out and meet with friends and family, the lost freedom of traveling) and were looking forward to whatever chance available to recover and return to their previous lifestyle and habits. Just as those who gamble are willing to do anything to make up for a loss, so probably those who were not entirely certain about the vaccine were more willing to take risks to recover the loss in quality of life. It follows that the pandemic emergency made people forgo some of their doubts about the vaccine when, at the same time, they had reasons to get their shot. In addition, several studies [ 19 , 55 , 56 ] have highlighted the relationship between anticipated regret and vaccination, showing that anticipated regret is associated with an increased likelihood of adhering, or having one's children adhere, to vaccine offerings. Trusting that the vaccine would work, focusing less on its potential side effects, made sense for people who were looking forward to recovering what was perceived (and was indeed) a loss of quality of life and freedom, because they desired to be back doing the things had ever enjoyed doing (e.g., going to restaurants, movies, etc.). This finding is also interesting from a communicative perspective: providing positive reasons that resonate well with people and have therefore a strong weight for them could offset their doubts, yielding to a greater acceptance of COVID-19 vaccination.

Therefore, it is crucial to consider what kind of reasons drive the decision toward or against vaccination. Allowing participants to openly report their reasons pro- or against- vaccination can facilitate a freer exploration of the concerns and reservations of the most hesitant individuals [ 24 ], thus providing valuable insights for shaping future vaccine-related communications. In fact, thanks to the regression tree on vaccination intention, it emerges that positive attitudes toward vaccines are strongly determined by "Protection" and "Community Protection" reasons. The fact that the sense of individual and collective protection is among the principal determinants of the decision with respect to COVID-19 vaccines suggests that in general vaccination is seen as a means of avoiding nefarious clinical consequences. The effect of the sense of communal protection as the reason favoring vaccination and of other-oriented S-PEC in determining the generation of multiple pro-vaccine motivations confirms previous results suggesting that people often are more willing to get vaccinated primarily to protect their loved ones [ 57 , 58 , 59 ], especially when they have a good understanding of how community immunity works [ 60 , 61 ]. However, it is worth mentioning that, at the time the study was conducted (March–April 2021), there was still uncertainty about whether COVID-19 vaccines could provide sterilizing immunity (i.e., could prevent the transmission of the infection) in addition to protecting the individual. To foster people's willingness to get vaccinated, it is crucial from a public health perspective that people understand that even when vaccines do not yield sterilizing immunity, vaccination can still increase protection of others by reducing the circulation of the virus.

The reasons that influenced the willingness to be vaccinated or the vaccination acceptance/booking were generally in line with the existing literature, although they differed depending on whether respondents had already been offered a vaccine or not: among those who did not received a vaccination offer, the main reasons promoting vaccination acceptance were protection against COVID-19 for oneself, one's family, friends, and community [ 23 ], while among the main reasons that reduced vaccination adherence for those who got the vaccine offer we found the lack of clinical trials [ 62 , 63 ], as well as the distrust of institutions and science [ 22 ]. This latter emerged as the most reported negative reason by those who have refused the vaccine and those who have not yet received the vaccine offer. Thus, effective communication aimed at defusing the perception of risk regarding vaccines themselves should focus on enhancing trust in the scientific process and experimental rigor. Indeed, these reasons were deemed as very important not only by those who refused the vaccination, but also by those who had not yet been offered the vaccine, and even by those who held mixed feelings but eventually chose to get vaccinated. While it is unlikely that individuals firmly against vaccination will be persuaded by simple interventions [ 64 ], we should keep in mind that vaccine hesitancy is a dynamic process. As such, reducing hesitancy or enhancing ambivalence, for example through motivational interviewing (e.g., [ 65 , 66 ]), could potentially lead to small shifts towards greater vaccine acceptance.

Our findings are also in line with the results of other international studies that have used a qualitative approach to examine reasons for and against vaccinations. For example, Hamilton and colleagues [ 67 ] employed a qualitative content analysis to extract the main motivations for and concerns about COVID-19 vaccination from medical records obtained by 102 consults in Australia. The study was conducted in June 2021, and revealed that most consults were driven by doubts about the vaccine available and recommended at that time (i.e., ChAdOx1-S, also known as Vaxzevria), followed by need for further information regarding vaccines and vaccination, also considering specific comorbidities. Notwithstanding the peculiarity of the Australian context in which a very low number of COVID-19 infections was observed, the analysis performed by Hamilton et al. [ 67 ] revealed a set of themes that largely overlaps with the reasons identified in our study. Indeed, among the reason to get vaccinated, 5 themes emerged: a) Protection, b) Occupational or facility responsibility or requirement, c) Trust in primary healthcare physician, d) Autonomy, and e) Civic duty, likewise, concerns about vaccination were mainly in terms of: a) Perceived vaccine risks, b) Perceived vaccine performance, c) Uncertainty, d) Autonomy, and e) Fairness in access. An aspect worth noting is that after the consultation, 81% of participants received the vaccination, 19% did not. Consistent results were observed in another study by Purvis and colleagues [ 68 ] conducted in the USA, which focused specifically on “hesitant adopters”, i.e. those who accepted vaccination but showed some level of hesitancy. To note that in this study the focus was on factors influencing the decision to get the COVID-19 vaccine, not on reasons against it. The authors interviewed 49 participants as a follow up of a larger study ( N  = 2022) conducted from mid-September 2021 through mid-October 2021, to explore factors that influenced their decision-making process about COVID-19 vaccination [ 68 ]. Two main themes emerged, each with four subthemes: 1) sociocultural context (political, cultural, health professionals, employment, and media environment) and 2) individual and group influences (attitudes and beliefs related to vaccines, family and social networks, free to return to normal, and COVID-19 outcomes).

As for the Italian context, to the best of our knowledge, only one study (i.e., [ 69 ]) attempted to provide a qualitative examination of the concept associated with vaccination in general, through open-ended and closed questions. Notably, this study was conducted a year later than our own study (April–May 2022) and was administered to a non-representative sample of Italians. The authors used a combination of closed and open-ended questions to assess concepts associated with vaccination in general. Consistent with our findings, Boragno et al. reported that participants who had been vaccinated against COVID-19 (92% of the sample) frequently mentioned concepts related to protection and salvation, whereas those who were not vaccinated frequently mentioned mistrust and ambivalence as concepts associated with vaccination [ 69 ].

This study has some limitations. First, COVID-19 perceived risk score was obtained only with respect to the disease and a similar score should be of interest for the COVID-19 vaccine. Second, data were collected during a vaccine offer limited to a well-defined slice of the population and the investigation on the vaccine acceptance/booking has, as a consequence, a limited sample size. Finally, the lack of a longitudinal perspective does not allow us to evaluate how strong the association is between the willingness to get vaccinated, vaccine acceptance and potential changes in risk perception. Thus, we cannot generalize our results beyond the period of data collection and to other countries or health systems. Since the dynamics have now changed, results may not apply to the decision to get a booster shot or not or an annual shot, however it might be interesting to study what motivations are most relevant now. Likewise, it remains to be established whether our results are generalisable to other populations.

Future studies could consider how the interaction between perceived risk associated with the disease and perceived risk associated with the vaccine influences the choice to get the shot. Furthermore, it would be important to explore how we can harness the reasons that most hold back vaccination in a specific communication strategy for the most hesitant people. Moreover, at the time of data collection, the vaccination campaign was still at an early stage, and only a small portion of the population had already received their shot. Therefore, we believe that it might be of particular interest to know more in detail, with a larger sample, what are the reasons that to date, almost 2 years after the release of the vaccine, still make some people reject the vaccine. Only by knowing these reasons will it be possible to develop appropriate vaccination campaigns.

In conclusion, our work examined pro- and against-vaccination reasons and how these, and their interaction, influence the decision to get vaccinated or not. Specifically, high emotional competence and risk perception influence the generation of pro- and against-vaccination reasons and that the presence of a strong pro-vaccination reason shifts intention toward vaccination. We also highlighted the category of reasons that influence intention to vaccinate. That said, given that the discussion about the next doses is still open and that in any case the next pandemic is a matter of when and not if [ 70 ], it is of paramount importance to know the best way to counteract vaccine hesitancy, fostering more effective communication strategies.

Availability of data and materials

Raw data are available on https://osf.io/dpn2q/?view_only=af05427467634411b471af7a8475ffab .

European Centre for Disease Prevention and Control. Cumulative uptake (%) of the primary course among adults (+18) in EU/EEA countries as of 2023–09–07. https://vaccinetracker.ecdc.europa.eu/public/extensions/COVID-19/vaccine-tracker.html#uptake-tab . Accessed 12 Sept 2023.

MacDonald NE. Vaccine hesitancy: Definition, scope and determinants. Vaccine. 2015; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vaccine.2015.04.036.

Bedford H, Attwell K, Danchin M, Marshall H, Corben P, Leask J. Vaccine hesitancy, refusal and access barriers: the need for clarity in terminology. Vaccine. 2018; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vaccine.2017.08.004.

Paul KT, Zimmermann BM, Corsico P, Fiske A, Geiger S, Johnson S, Kuiper JM, Lievevrouw E, Marelli L, Prainsack B, Spahl W. Anticipating hopes, fears and expectations towards COVID-19 vaccines: a qualitative interview study in seven European countries. SSM-Qual Res Health. 2022; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssmqr.2021.100035.

Murphy J, Vallières F, Bentall RP, Shevlin M, McBride O, Hartman TK, McKay R, Bennett K, Mason L, Gibson-Miller J, Levita L. Psychological characteristics associated with COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy and resistance in Ireland and the United Kingdom. Nat Commun. 2021; https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-20226-9.

Soares P, Rocha JV, Moniz M, Gama A, Laires PA, Pedro AR, Dias S, Leite A, Nunes C. Factors associated with COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy. Vaccines. 2021; https://doi.org/10.3390/vaccines9030300.

Fisher KA, Bloomstone SJ, Walder J, Crawford S, Fouayzi H, Mazor KM. Attitudes toward a potential SARS-CoV-2 vaccine: a survey of US adults. Ann Internal Med. 2020; https://doi.org/10.7326/M20-3569.

Robertson E, Reeve KS, Niedzwiedz CL, Moore J, Blake M, Green M, Katikireddi SV, Benzeval MJ. Predictors of COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy in the UK household longitudinal study. Brain Behav Immun. 2021; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2021.03.008.

Caserotti M, Gavaruzzi T, Girardi P, Tasso A, Buizza C, Candini V, Zarbo C, Chiarotti F, Brescianini S, Calamandrei G, Starace F. Who is likely to vacillate in their COVID-19 vaccination decision? Free-riding intention and post-positive reluctance. Prev Med. 2022; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2021.106885.

Caserotti M, Girardi P, Tasso A, Rubaltelli E, Lotto L, Gavaruzzi T. Joint analysis of the intention to vaccinate and to use contact tracing app during the COVID-19 pandemic. Sci Rep. 2022; https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-04765-9 .

Pereira B, Fehl AG, Finkelstein SR, Jiga‐Boy GM, Caserotti M. Scarcity in COVID‐19 vaccine supplies reduces perceived vaccination priority and increases vaccine hesitancy. Psychol Mark. 2022; https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.21629 .

Đorđević JM, Mari S, Vdović M, Milošević A. Links between conspiracy beliefs, vaccine knowledge, and trust: Anti-vaccine behavior of Serbian adults. Soc Sci Med. 2021; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.113930 .

Candini V, Brescianini S, Chiarotti F, Zarbo C, Zamparini M, Caserotti M, Gavaruzzi T, Girardi P, Lotto L, Tasso A, Starace F. Conspiracy mentality and health-related behaviour during the COVID-19 pandemic: a multi-wave survey in Italy. Public Health. 2023, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.puhe.2022.11.005 .

Asch DA, Baron J, Hershey JC, Kunreuther H, Meszaros J, Ritov I, Spranca M. Omission bias and pertussis vaccination. Medic Decis Mak. 1994; https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X9401400204 .

Meszaros JR, Asch DA, Baron J, Hershey JC, Kunreuther H, Schwartz-Buzaglo J. Cognitive processes and the decisions of some parents to forego pertussis vaccination for their children. J Clin Epidemiol. 1996; https://doi.org/10.1016/0895-4356(96)00007-8 .

Bell RA, McGlone MS, Dragojevic M. Vicious viruses and vigilant vaccines: Effects of linguistic agency assignment in health policy advocacy. J Health Commun. 2014; https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2013.81133 .

Nan X, Madden K. HPV vaccine information in the blogosphere: how positive and negative blogs influence vaccine-related risk perceptions, attitudes, and behavioral intentions. Health Commun. 2012; https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2012.661348 .

Christy SM, Winger JG, Raffanello EW, Halpern LF, Danoff-Burg S, Mosher CE. The role of anticipated regret and health beliefs in HPV vaccination intentions among young adults. J Behav Med. 2016; https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-016-9716-z .

Chapman GB, Coups EJ. Emotions and preventive health behavior: worry, regret, and influenza vaccination. Health Psychol. 2006; https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.25.1.82 .

Klasko-Foster LB, Przybyla S, Orom H, Gage-Bouchard E, Kiviniemi MT. The influence of affect on HPV vaccine decision making in an HPV vaccine naïve college student population. Prev Med Rep. 2020; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pmedr.2020.101195.

World Health Organization = Organisation mondiale de la Santé. Understanding the behavioural and social drivers of vaccine uptake WHO position paper – May 2022 – Comprendre les facteurs comportementaux et sociaux de l’adoption des vaccins Note de synthèse de l’OMS – mai 2022. Weekly Epidemiological Record = Relevé épidémiologique hebdomadaire, 97 (20), 209 - 224. World Health Organization = Organisation mondiale de la Santé. 2022; https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/354460 .

Fieselmann J, Annac K, Erdsiek F, Yilmaz-Aslan Y, Brzoska P. What are the reasons for refusing a COVID-19 vaccine? A qualitative analysis of social media in Germany. BMC Public Health. 2022; https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-022-13265-y .

Moore R, Purvis RS, Hallgren E, Willis DE, Hall S, Reece S, CarlLee S, Judkins H, McElfish PA. Motivations to vaccinate among hesitant adopters of the COVID-19 vaccine. J Commun Health. 2022; https://doi.org/10.1007/s10900-021-01037-5 .

Cassels TG, Birch SA. Comparisons of an open-ended vs. forced-choice ‘mind reading’task: Implications for measuring perspective-taking and emotion recognition. PLoS One. 2014; https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093653 .

Kahneman D, & Tversky A. On the interpretation of intuitive probability: A reply to Jonathan Cohen. 1979; https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(79)90024-6 .

Tversky A, Kahneman D. Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. J Risk Uncertain. 1992; https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00122574 .

Baumeister RF, Bratslavsky E, Finkenauer C, Vohs KD. Bad is stronger than good. Review of general psychology. 2001; https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323 .

Chor JS, Ngai KL, Goggins WB, Wong MC, Wong SY, Lee N, Leung TF, Rainer TH, Griffiths S, Chan PK. Willingness of Hong Kong healthcare workers to accept pre-pandemic influenza vaccination at different WHO alert levels: two questionnaire surveys. BMJ. 2009; https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.b3391 .

Pareek M, Clark T, Dillon H, Kumar R, Stephenson I. Willingness of healthcare workers to accept voluntary stockpiled H5N1 vaccine in advance of pandemic activity. Vaccine. 2009; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vaccine.2008.12.006 .

Viswanath K, Bekalu M, Dhawan D, Pinnamaneni R, Lang J, McLoud R. Individual and social determinants of COVID-19 vaccine uptake. BMC Public Health. 2021; https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-10862-1 .

Caserotti M, Girardi P, Rubaltelli E, Tasso A, Lotto L, Gavaruzzi T. Associations of COVID-19 risk perception with vaccine hesitancy over time for Italian residents. Soc Sci Med. 2021; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.113688 .

Finucane ML, Peters E, & Slovic P. (2003). Judgment and decision making: The dance of affect and reason. In: S. L. Schneider & J. Shanteau, editors. Emerging Perspectives on Judgment and Decision Research Cambridge. University Press; 2003. 327–364. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511609978.012 .

Pittarello A, Conte B, Caserotti M, Scrimin S, Rubaltelli E. Emotional intelligence buffers the effect of physiological arousal on dishonesty. Psychonomic Bull Rev. 2018; https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1285-9 .

Scrimin S, Rubaltelli E. Dehumanization after terrorism: the role of psychophysiological emotion regulation and trait emotional intelligence. Curr Psychol. 2021; https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144‐019‐00189‐x .

Tomljenovic H, Bubic A, Erceg N. It just doesn’t feel right–the relevance of emotions and intuition for parental vaccine conspiracy beliefs and vaccination uptake. Psychol Health. 2020; https://doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2019.1673894 .

Gavaruzzi T, Caserotti M, Leo I, Tasso A, Speri L, Ferro A, Fretti E, Sannino A, Rubaltelli E, Lotto L. The role of emotional competences in parents’ vaccine hesitancy. Vaccines. 2021; https://doi.org/10.3390/vaccines9030298 .

ISTAT. Resident population on 1st January: By age. http://dati.istat.it/?lang=en&SubSessionId=d7024c9e-239b-455d-924b-df19345a27b2 . Accessed Sept 25, 2023.

Mikolajczak M., Brasseur S, & Fantini-Hauwel C. Measuring intrapersonal and interpersonal EQ: The short profile of emotional competence (S-PEC). Pers Individ Differ. 2014; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.023 .

Olmos A, Govindasamy P. A practical guide for using propensity score weighting in R. Pract Assess Res Eval. 2015; https://doi.org/10.7275/jjtm-r398 .

Smithson M, Verkuilen J. A better lemon squeezer? Maximum-likelihood regression with beta-distributed dependent variables. Psychological methods. 2006; https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.11.1.54 .

Ferrari S, Cribari-Neto F. Beta regression for modelling rates and proportions. Journal of applied statistics. 2004; https://doi.org/10.1080/0266476042000214501 .

Thernau T, Atkinson B, Ripley B. Rpart: Recursive Partitioning. R Package 4.1–0. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=rpart .

RC Team. R Core Team R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R. . Foundation for Statistical Computing. 2014. https://www.r-project.org .

Freeman D, Loe BS, Chadwick A, Vaccari C, Waite F, Rosebrock L, ... & Lambe S. COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy in the UK: the Oxford coronavirus explanations, attitudes, and narratives survey (Oceans) II. Psychol Med. 2022; https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720005188 .

Caserotti M, Gavaruzzi T, Girardi P, Sellaro R, Rubaltelli E, Tasso A, Lotto L. People’s perspectives about COVID-19 vaccination certificate: Findings from a representative Italian sample. Vaccine. 2022; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vaccine.2022.08.016 .

MacDonald NE, Comeau J, Dubé È, Graham J, Greenwood M, Harmon S, McElhaney J, Meghan McMurtry C, Middleton A, Steenbeek A, Taddio A. Royal society of Canada COVID-19 report: Enhancing COVID-19 vaccine acceptance in Canada. Facets. 2021; https://doi.org/10.1139/facets-2021-0037 .

Schwarzinger M, Watson V, Arwidson P, Alla F, Luchini S. COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy in a representative working-age population in France: a survey experiment based on vaccine characteristics. Lancet Public Health. 2021; https://doi.org/10.1016/S2468-2667(21)00012-8 .

Slovic P, Finucane M, Peters E, MacGregor DG. Rational actors or rational fools: Implications of the affect heuristic for behavioral economics. J Socio-Econ. 2002; https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-5357(02)00174-9 .

Slovic P, Finucane ML, Peters E, MacGregor DG. Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk and rationality. In The feeling of risk 2013 Mar 7 (pp. 21–36). Routledge.

Bhopal SS, Bagaria J, Olabi B, Bhopal R. Children and young people remain at low risk of COVID-19 mortality. Lancet Child Adolesc Health. 2021; https://doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(21)00066-3 .

Lazarus JV, Wyka K, Rauh L, Rabin K, Ratzan S, Gostin LO, Larson HJ, El-Mohandes A. Hesitant or not? The association of age, gender, and education with potential acceptance of a COVID-19 vaccine: a country-level analysis. J Health Commun. 2020; https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2020.1868630 .

Seale H, Heywood AE, Leask J, Sheel M, Durrheim DN, Bolsewicz K, Kaur R. Examining Australian public perceptions and behaviors towards a future COVID-19 vaccine. BMC Infect Dis. 2021; https://doi.org/10.1186/s12879-021-05 .

Butter S, McGlinchey E, Berry E, Armour C. Psychological, social, and situational factors associated with COVID‐19 vaccination intentions: A study of UK key workers and non‐key workers. Br J Health Psychol. 2022; https://doi.org/10.1111/bjhp.12530 .

Freedman DA, Berk RA. Weighting regressions by propensity scores. Eval Rev. 2008; https://doi.org/10.1177/0193841X08317586 .

Lagoe C, Farrar KM. Are you willing to risk it? The relationship between risk, regret, and vaccination intent. Psychol Health Med. 2015; https://doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2014.911923 .

Ziarnowski KL, Brewer NT, Weber B. Present choices, future outcomes: anticipated regret and HPV vaccination. Prev Med. 2009; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2008.10.006 .

Betsch C, Böhm R, Korn L, Holtmann C. On the benefits of explaining herd immunity in vaccine advocacy. Nat Hum Behav. 2017; https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0056 .

Loomba S, de Figueiredo A, Piatek SJ, de Graaf K, Larson HJ. Measuring the impact of COVID-19 vaccine misinformation on vaccination intent in the UK and USA. Nat Hum Behav. 2021; https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01056-1 .

Pfattheicher S, Petersen MB, Böhm R. Information about herd immunity through vaccination and empathy promote COVID-19 vaccination intentions. Health Psychol. 2022;41(2):85.

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Hakim H, Provencher T, Chambers CT, Driedger SM, Dube E, Gavaruzzi T, ... & Witteman HO. Interventions to help people understand community immunity: a systematic review. Vaccine. 2019; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vaccine.2018.11.016 .

Hakim H, Bettinger JA, Chambers CT, Driedger SM, Dubé E, Gavaruzzi T, Giguere AMC, Kavanagh É, Leask J, MacDonald SE, Orji R, Parent E, Paquette J, Roberge J, Sander B, Scherer AM, Tremblay-Breault M, Wilson K, Reinharz D, Witteman HO. A Web Application About Herd Immunity Using Personalized Avatars: Development Study. Journal of medical Internet research. 2020; https://doi.org/10.2196/20113 .

Callaghan T, Moghtaderi A, Lueck JA, Hotez P, Strych U, Dor A, Fowler EF, Motta M. Correlates and disparities of intention to vaccinate against COVID-19. Soc Sci Med (1982). 2021; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.113638 .

Griffith J, Marani H, Monkman H. COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy in Canada: Content analysis of tweets using the theoretical domains framework. Journal of medical Internet research. 2021; https://doi.org/10.2196/26874 .

Attwell K, Lake J, Sneddon J, Gerrans P, Blyth C, & Lee J. Converting the maybes: Crucial for a successful COVID-19 vaccination strategy. PLoS One. 2021; https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0245907 .

Breckenridge LA, Burns D, & Nye C. The use of motivational interviewing to overcome COVID‐19 vaccine hesitancy in primary care settings. Public Health Nurs. 2022; https://doi.org/10.1111/phn.13003 .

Gabarda A, & Butterworth SW. Using best practices to address COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy: The case for the motivational interviewing approach. Health Promot Pract, 2021; https://doi.org/10.1177/152483992110164 .

Hamilton EM, Oversby S, Ratsch A, & Kitchener S.COVID-19 vaccination: An exploratory study of the motivations and concerns detailed in the medical records of a regional Australian population. Vaccines. 2022; https://doi.org/10.3390/vaccines10050657 .

Purvis RS, Moore R, Willis DE, Hallgren E, & McElfish PA. Factors influencing COVID-19 vaccine decision-making among hesitant adopters in the United States. Human Vaccines Immunother. 2022; https://doi.org/10.1080/21645515.2022.2114701 .

Boragno P, Fiabane E, Taino I, Maffoni M, Sommovigo V, Setti I, Gabanelli P. Perceptions of COVID-19 Vaccines: Protective Shields or Threatening Risks? A Descriptive Exploratory Study among the Italian Population. Vaccines. 2023; https://doi.org/10.3390/vaccines11030642 .

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Why it matters: The pandemic threat. Retrieved December. 2020;1:2020.

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Open access funding provided by Università degli Studi di Padova. The project was developed thanks to institutional research funding of TG and AT.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialization, University of Padova, Padua, Italy

Marta Caserotti, Roberta Sellaro, Enrico Rubaltelli & Lorella Lotto

Department of Environmental Sciences, Informatics and Statistics, Ca’ Foscari University of Venezia, Venice, Italy

Paolo Girardi

Department of Humanities, University of Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy

Alessandra Tasso

Department of Medical and Surgical Sciences, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy

Teresa Gavaruzzi

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

MC: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Visualization, Writing—original draft and Writing—review & editing. PG: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Visualization, Writing—original draft. RS: Conceptualization, Writing—review & editing. ER: Conceptualization, Writing—review & editing. AT: Conceptualization, Writing—review & editing. LL: Conceptualization, Writing—review & editing. TG: Conceptualization, Visualization, Writing—review & editing.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marta Caserotti .

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate.

The project was approved by the ethical committee for Psychology Research of the University of Padova (Italy), with protocol number 3911/2020, and informed consent was obtained for all participants.

Consent for publication

Competing interests.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: appendix 1..

Scoring for pro- and against-vaccination reasons.  Appendix 2. Structure of the questionnaire. Table S1. Selection criteria. Table S2. Number of items, internal consistency (Cronbach’s α), name of the items and their estimated loadings, total deviance explained by the loadings and proportion of variance explained by EFA for COVID-19 perceived risk. Table S3. Odds ratios (ORs) estimated by the logistic model for the propensity score weighting for the COVID-19 vaccine offer. Table S4 . Predicted willingness to get vaccinated by combination of pro- and against-vaccination reasons by category of reference.  Table S5. Frequency of reported categories of pro- and against-vaccination reasons overall, and by COVID-19 vaccine status. Figure S1. Distribution of the propensity scores by vaccine offer.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ . The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver ( http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/ ) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Caserotti, M., Girardi, P., Sellaro, R. et al. To vaccinate or not to vaccinate? The interplay between pro- and against- vaccination reasons. BMC Public Health 23 , 2207 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-023-17112-6

Download citation

Received : 12 December 2022

Accepted : 30 October 2023

Published : 09 November 2023

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-023-17112-6

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Pro-and against-reasons
  • Vaccination intention
  • Risk perception
  • Emotional competences

BMC Public Health

ISSN: 1471-2458

argumentative research paper on vaccines

IMAGES

  1. ≫ Vaccinations: Are There any Real Benefits? Free Essay Sample on

    argumentative research paper on vaccines

  2. 10 page argumentative research paper

    argumentative research paper on vaccines

  3. 😍 Good argumentative research essay topics. 30 Good Argumentative

    argumentative research paper on vaccines

  4. What it Takes to Create a Vaccine

    argumentative research paper on vaccines

  5. Should COVID-19 Vaccines Be Mandatory?

    argumentative research paper on vaccines

  6. Argument paper to buy

    argumentative research paper on vaccines

VIDEO

  1. Where to Find Approved Articles for Your Research Paper

  2. Argumentative Essay Research A

  3. Argumentative Research Tutorial

  4. Argumentative Essay Research, Fall 2023

  5. PEALzilla

  6. How can vaccine advocates support good journalism?

COMMENTS

  1. A Vaccine a Day to Keep the Doctor Away: A Research Essay on

    An additional aspect of vaccines many parents are troubled with is the increase in suggested vaccines for young children. “Today, the CDC recommends that children receive vaccines for 10 diseases — plus the flu vaccine — by age 6, which can mean up to 37 separate shots. That compares to five vaccines for the same age group in 1995 ...

  2. Attitudes on voluntary and mandatory vaccination against

    Several vaccines against COVID-19 have now been developed and are already being rolled out around the world. The decision whether or not to get vaccinated has so far been left to the individual citizens. However, there are good reasons, both in theory as well as in practice, to believe that the willingness to get vaccinated might not be sufficiently high to achieve herd immunity. A policy of ...

  3. To vaccinate or not to vaccinate? The interplay between pro

    Background By mid 2023, European countries reached 75% of vaccine coverage for COVID-19 and although vaccination rates are quite high, many people are still hesitant. A plethora of studies have investigated factors associated with COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy, however, insufficient attention has been paid to the reasons why people get vaccinated against COVID-19. Our work aims to investigate the ...