Autism Speaks

  • Common misconceptions
  • Set Your Location
  • Learn the signs
  • Symptoms of autism
  • What causes autism?
  • Asperger syndrome
  • Autism statistics and facts
  • Learn about screening
  • Screening questionnaire
  • First Concern to Action
  • Autism diagnosis criteria: DSM-5
  • Newly diagnosed
  • Associated conditions
  • Sensory issues
  • Interventions
  • Access services
  • Caregiver Skills Training (CST)
  • Information by topic
  • Resource Guide
  • Autism Response Team
  • Our mission
  • Our grantmaking
  • Research programs
  • Autism by the Numbers
  • Fundraising & events
  • World Autism Month
  • Social fundraising
  • Ways to give
  • Memorial & tributes
  • Workplace giving
  • Corporate partnership
  • Become a partner
  • Ways to engage
  • Meet our Partners
  • Deteccion de autismo
  • Deteccion temprana
  • My Autism Guide
  • Select Your Location

Please enter your location to help us display the correct information for your area.

Teaching children with nonverbal autism to read

Dr. Charlotte DiStefano

This week’s “Got Questions?” response is by psychologist Charlotte DiStefano, an Autism Speaks Meixner Postdoctoral Fellow in Translational Science, at the University of California, Los Angeles. Dr. DiStefano’s Autism Speak Meixner Fellowship in Translational Research involves identifying brain activity patterns that relate to language and literacy abilities in minimally verbal children with autism. The hope is to guide the development of individualized therapy and educational programs that best address each child’s needs. 

Any suggestions for teaching a nonverbal 8-year-old to read? He can form sentences using augmentative communication.

I’m so glad you’ve asked this question. Many people wrongly assume that children who don’t speak can’t learn to read. But that’s definitely not true!

male teacher reading a book to students

Two years ago, I published the results of a small study on the effectiveness of a reading program adapted for minimally verbal 5 and 6 year olds . All the students showed increased story comprehension and engagement. Unfortunately we have little other research on reading ability – or literacy – among minimally verbal kids with autism. As a result, we really don’t know how many nonverbal or minimally verbal children with autism can read or have the ability to learn how to read.

But many parents and professionals can tell you of children who can read despite not using spoken language. So we know it’s possible. What I find especially amazing is that many of these children seem to figure out how to read on their own – because no one ever gave them direct literacy instruction. 

I’ve worked with several minimally verbal and non-verbal kids who showed me that they could read. They did so in various ways: matching words and sentences to pictures, typing words and/or following written text with a finger as an adult reads a book.

One of the major challenges to teaching reading to minimally verbal kids is that traditional literacy instruction relies heavily on spoken language. As you’ll probably recall from your own first grade experience, learning to read usually involves a big focus on phonics: Teachers have children enunciate the sounds of letters. Then the children learn how these sounds combine to form words. 

For kids who use no or minimal spoken language, this obviously presents some difficulty.  How can a child learn to “sound out” words when he or she has difficulty making sounds? 

Fortunately there are many ways to teach reading that don’t depend on a child using spoken language.

How to teach nonverbal children to read

1. spend lots of time reading with your child.

First and foremost, I recommend spending lots of time reading with your child! We know that reading with children encourages their development of language and reading.

One of the most beneficial aspects of shared reading is the dialogue between the adult and child, as they discuss the book that they are reading. Although minimally verbal kids won’t be able to have a verbal conversation about the book, they can very much engage non-verbally with the book and the reader.

elephants on a page from a verbal conversation book

2. Nonverbal interactive reading

As you read with your child, give him opportunities to interact nonverbally.

Here are some activities you can share as you read:

  • Run your finger just under the text as you read. Then ask your child do the pointing.
  • Ask your child to turn the pages at the right time.
  • Give your child some story props so he can act out the story as it unfolds.
  • Take turns imitating what the characters are doing.

These and similar activities will help your child engage with a book without relying on spoken language. 

3. Discuss stories using assisted communication

AAC device used to help while reading a book

It’s great to hear that your child uses an augmentative and assistive communication (AAC) device. The device can provide your child with added opportunities to interact with you and the book you’re reading together. Before reading a new book, make sure that the system has a good array of symbols related to the story. For example, if you’re reading a book about a birthday party, make sure you have downloaded symbols for, say, “party,” “presents,” “cake” and “balloons.” As you read a story or book, use the symbols to discuss the characters and actions. 

The AAC system itself offers great opportunities for developing literacy. Make sure that the device is set to display the words that go with each picture symbol. This will help your child associate written words with objects and actions.

As your child becomes more familiar with the printed words, I suggest gradually reducing the size of the accompanying pictures while increasing the size of the text. Once he’s comfortably recognizing the text, you may be able to remove the pictures entirely! 

4. Reading and writing with speech-generating devices

screen of a speech generating device used to help with reading and writing

The typing function on speech-generating devices is another wonderful avenue for developing literacy. I’ve worked with minimally verbal children who, after watching me program new words into their device, suddenly start choosing their own symbols and typing in what they want to say! 

Since we didn’t know these kids could read, we were certainly surprised! And once they figured out that they could type in words of their choosing, they became much more independent communicators. 

Though your child may not yet be able to type and spell words, let him watch as you program new words into the device. Explain what you’re doing. You never know when he’ll surprise you and start typing the words he wants to say.

5. Practice literacy when you’re out and about

I also encourage you to read signs with your child – especially safety-related signage. This can become a natural part of your day when you walk and drive through your community. Reading a stop sign is an important example. The same goes for the street sign or signs that will help your child identify the block where you live. Then there are crosswalk signs, exit and entrance signs. You get the idea.

Putting it all together

To summarize, we can all support literacy development in minimally verbal and non-verbal kids with autism by:

  • reading together
  • giving them opportunities to interact with a story or other written information in whatever way they’re able 
  • teaching them to recognize words paired with pictures or symbols
  • showing them how we set up symbols in their AAC devices and
  • giving them opportunities to type their own words when they’re ready 
  • reading signage – especially safety signage – when out in the community

I want to thank you again for your question. I hope these tips prove helpful for you, your son and many other readers.

Additional Resources & Tools

Andy Shih, Chief Science Officer at Autism Speaks, and Sabrina Vaughn, Founder of the Hatching Hope Foundation

AAC: A complete guide for parents of autistic children

  • Beaming Health
  • Learning Center

Speech Therapy

Augmentative and alternative communication (aac): what to know.

Updated: January 4, 2024 · 7 Minute Read

Alex Hurtado

Reviewed by:

Alex Hurtado

  • Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) is a powerful tool that can transform the lives of people who have difficulty with speech.
  • AAC is especially important for autistic children, because it gives them a way to communicate without speech.
  • AAC can ensure your child always has a way to communicate, allowing for fluctuating communication needs. This helps keep your child safe, promotes autonomy, connection, and meaningful interactions.
  • AAC comes in many types and forms, including gestures, facial expressions, writing, drawing, pointing, and high-tech devices. AAC should be implemented in a way that respects and supports your child’s unique communication style.
  • 1. What is AAC?
  • 2. Why is AAC important for autistic kids?
  • 3. Types of AAC
  • 4. Will using AAC stop my child from talking?
  • 5. How can I use AAC for my child?
  • 6. How do I get my child an AAC device?
  • 7. All communication is valid
  • 8. Your child has something to say
  • 9. Conclusion

What is AAC?

Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) is any type of non-spoken communication. Augmentative means adding to someone’s speech. Alternative means that it will be used instead of speech. When you use something other than spoken words to express a thought, feeling, or idea, you’re using AAC! All humans use AAC every day — think about things like gestures, drawing pictures, writing, or facial expressions.

Why is AAC important for autistic kids?

We all have different ways we communicate best. For example, the person writing this article prefers written communication for some things and speaking for other things.

Speech may be difficult for autistic kids for many reasons. AAC gives all people an alternative way to communicate that doesn’t require speech. Autism is a dynamic disability, which means your child’s needs and abilities may fluctuate from day to day or moment to moment. Even neurotypical people sometimes have days when they feel drained and don’t feel up to talking. It is crucial to your child’s safety that they always have a way to communicate.

Types of AAC

Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) can come in many forms. Your child may use AAC for a little while, for their whole lives, or only sometimes. The type of AAC your child uses and the frequency will depend on their unique needs and preferences. Let’s explore the different types of AAC.

AAC can be aided or unaided. Aided AAC requires help from someone or something else, like holding a letter board. Unaided AAC means your child doesn’t need anything but their own body to use it, like sign language.

AAC can also be low-tech or high-tech. Low tech would be options like picture boards. High-tech can be things like speech-generating devices.

  • Gestures. This can include pointing, pulling you toward something, making shapes or expressions with their hands or body. Your child may grab your hand and pull you toward the door when they want to go outside, or put the remote in your hand when they want you to turn the TV on. All of these gestures are one way your child communicates.
  • Facial expressions. Sad faces, happy faces, all the faces! Facial expressions are often a way to express feelings. Keep in mind, autistic kids may interpret facial expressions differently than neurotypical people. Their own facial expressions may not match what you’d typically expect. For example, you may think your child looks angry, when really they are sad or disappointed.
  • Writing and/or typing. Many nonspeaking autistic people prefer to communicate through writing or typing. Even some speaking neurodivergent people prefer written communication. Writing is a less stressful way to communicate because it allows our brains time to process information and create a thoughtful reply. Spoken communication can feel overwhelming, and has a lot of nuance and social cues we may not pick up on. Reading something might make sense when hearing or listening to something doesn’t. But with writing/typing, we can express ourselves at a pace and in a way that feels more intuitive to us. Some autistic children will learn to read or write very young (hyperlexia).
  • Drawing. Art can be a powerful tool of communication . Your child may find that they express themselves better sometimes through drawing or creating art than through speech or writing. Many autistic people, like author and scientist Temple Grandin , are visual thinkers. This means the person thinks and learns best in pictures. (Think engineers, artists, and designers.) For an example of this, check out Viktor , a teenage autistic boy who doesn’t speak but communicates through his stunning art.
  • Spelling words by pointing to letters. This AAC uses a letter board and the person’s pointing finger or object (like a pencil) to point to the letter the person wants as they spell out words to form sentences. This can be a great option for children with autism who don’t have the fine motor skills to write or type. One example of this communication system is Spelling 2 Communicate (S2C). There is some disagreement about S2C and programs like it. It’s a complex debate, but Not An Autism Mom breaks it down here .
  • Pointing to photos, pictures, or written words. One common system used in ABA is PECS (Picture Exchange Communication System). It’s a very rigid system that mostly focuses on requests (“I want”, “I need”, etc.). PCES isn’t a good long term communication solution. Not all picture communication systems are PECS. Visual aids can be extremely helpful for autistic kids, whether they talk a little or a lot. You can use pictures in a variety of ways to help your child communicate. Pictures are a great part of a communication system. Autistic children need more robust communication systems .
  • Using an app on an iPad or tablet to communicate. This is one type of high-tech AAC device. Many kinds of AAC devices exist, ranging in cost and complexity. You can purchase some devices online, or you can get them through your child’s insurance provider. There are some PECS-style apps for tablets, but there are also more advanced systems. For many nonverbal adults and kids, their AAC device is a lifeline.
  • Using a computer device that “speaks” for them. This is sometimes called a “speech-generating device.” The high-tech device allows the user to input text or select a preset option, and the device will say the word or phrase for them. You may be familiar with the speech-generating device that Dr. Stephen Hawking famously used throughout his career. Speech-generating devices can offer flexibility, opportunity, and robust communication. While there is a slight delay in creating the speech and the computer speaking it, this AAC allows the user to converse with others in real time.

Will using AAC stop my child from talking?

No! In fact, AAC can actually help your child speak more! Your child will continue to develop language and speech. AAC is a tool to support your child’s communication. Children will naturally use speech when they are able. AAC is there to enhance your child’s abilities.

How can I use AAC for my child?

Whether your child is verbal or nonspeaking, autistic or even neurotypical, there are so many ways you can incorporate AAC into your child’s life! Here are some tips:

  • Work with an SLP (Speech Language Pathologist) who knows their stuff. We highly recommend working with a qualified and knowledgeable speech therapist . They can help you develop a robust, appropriate communication system for your child. Your child’s personalized communication system should be unique to them. For example, it may look like a combination of pictures, an AAC device, gestures (like sign language), and more. Find a speech therapist who understands and will work with the way your child processes language.
  • Create an environment that respects their communication style. It’s important to understand that speakers overtake nonspeakers in conversation and everyday interactions. Society as a whole is biased toward speaking people. This means the burden will likely fall to your child to make themselves heard — which is a big task, especially for a child. It’s vital that you create an environment where your child’s communication style is respected. That means providing space and time for them to listen to and understand what others are saying. Then time for them to think, create, and share a response. It also means acknowledging ALL their communication as valid and important (including behavior).
  • Focus on connection and autonomy. The whole point of communication is so your child can express their needs, assert their independence, form connections with people, and have meaningful social interactions. Your child’s AAC should add value to all these areas of their lives. Learning or using AAC should never be about drilling, correction, or getting it right. The focus should be on your child’s strengths, promoting their autonomy, and adding support wherever your child needs it. When adding AAC to your child’s life, always prioritize connection over correction .

How do I get my child an AAC device?

You can get your child an AAC device a few ways.

  • Add it to your child’s IEP. If your child has an IEP, you can request that an AAC device be made part of it, and that they always have access to it in school. (Some schools will allow you to take the device home, too.)
  • Ask your child’s doctor. You can request an evaluation for an AAC device through your child’s doctor. This requires an evaluation, and your child’s insurance should cover part (or all) of the costs.
  • Buy one yourself. Many AAC devices are just apps installed on regular iPads or a tablet. They can be a bit pricey, but getting insurance to pay for AAC devices can take a long time.

Not An Autism Mom’s guide to getting your child an AAC device explains each of those options in more detail. Remember, a speech therapist can help you figure out which AAC device is best for your child and teach them how to use it.

All communication is valid

Unfortunately, many kids are not offered AAC or are denied access to AAC by educators or therapists. Sometimes the adults in a nonverbal child’s life will insist they speak, even if that is not what is most comfortable for them. This can cause a lot of stress on the child, and denies them their right to communication. Jordyn Zimmerman is a nonspeaking autistic person who was denied AAC as a child , but thrived once she had access to it. She’s now on the board of Communication First , a member of President Biden’s committee , and holds a Master’s degree.

Whatever way your child communicates best is worthy of respect. No one should be forcing your child to speak, or denying them access to AAC.

Your child has something to say

We must always presume our children have something to say, even if they cannot speak. The same applies for AAC. We should always presume competence when thinking about AAC. There’s no special training or requirements for a child to learn AAC. Your child doesn’t even have to be a certain age! Assume that your child has something to say and can learn how to share it. Check out AssistiveWare’s guide to presuming competence about AAC for more information.

Whether your child is verbal or nonverbal, understanding and respecting autistic communication is important. AAC can be life-changing, and all people deserve access to it. Using AAC doesn’t make anyone less smart, less worthy, or less capable than anyone else. We all use AAC every day without even realizing it! All ways of communicating are valid and should be respected. For information, check out the deep dives below.

Get our best articles delivered to your inbox each month.

We respect your privacy .

Dive Deeper

Article references.

  • ASHA. Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC). Asha.org. Published 2009. https://www.asha.org/public/speech/disorders/aac/
  • American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC). American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. Published 2023. https://www.asha.org/NJC/AAC/
  • Disability Justice. Abuse and Exploitation of People with Developmental Disabilities | Disability Justice. Disability Justice. Published 2014. https://disabilityjustice.org/justice-denied/abuse-and-exploitation/
  • USSAAC. AAC Devices. https://ussaac.org/aac-info/aac-devices/
  • USSAAC. Fact Sheet. Accessed June 26, 2023. https://ussaac.org/aac-info/aac-awareness/factsheet/
  • Montgomery C. Dear Parents Who Want to Keep Their Nonspeaking Children Safe as They Go Out Into the World. CommunicationFIRST. Accessed June 26, 2023. https://communicationfirst.org/dear-parents-who-want-to-keep-their-nonspeaking-children-safe-as-they-go-out-into-the-world/
  • Skenazy L. Severely autistic kids use miracle tool to communicate for first time. Published December 24, 2022. https://nypost.com/2022/12/24/severely-autistic-kids-use-miracle-tool-to-communicate-for-first-time/
  • Breglia E. What did you say?! Helping children who are hard to understand. High Hopes. Published April 1, 2020. Accessed June 26, 2023. https://www.highhopesdubai.com/blog/what-did-you-say-helping-children-who-are-hard-to-understand/
  • admin. The positive impact of AAC on Speech and Language Development. High Hopes. Published March 2, 2021. Accessed June 26, 2023. https://www.highhopesdubai.com/the-positive-impact-of-aac-on-speech-and-language-development/

Alex Hurtado is a freelance writer with nearly 10 years of experience. She has worked in and written for a wide range of industries including education, politics, business, and entertainment, just to name a few.

Jeryn Cambrah

Written by:

Jeryn Cambrah

Jeryn Cambrah is a neurodivergent writer, artist, poet, author, and mother of an autistic child. She's passionate about equipping parents with the knowledge they need to help their children thrive. Join her in the Infinity Moms Facebook group!

Did you find this article helpful?

Other articles related to speech therapy.

Find our most popular articles in each topic.

For Providers

Parent tips, diagnostic evaluations, aba therapy, schools & ieps, other therapies, our favorites, secure attachment, find child development providers by city.

We have the most comprehensive and up-to-date directory in the country of child development providers.

International Society for Augmentative and Alternative Communication

  • About ISAAC
  • AAC Awareness
  • AAC Emergency Support
  • Communication Access
  • Conference History
  • Publications
  • Online Store
  • Members Only
  • Dis/Ableism in AAC?! What are you talking about?
  • “Show Me YOUR AAC Sensitivity” and “I’ll Show You My Voice” – Communication in Health Care Settings
  • video on demand thumbnail list test page
  • Casting Release for Out of My Mind movie
  • Webinar Registration Confirmation
  • Information
  •  »  Online Store
  •  »  Books
  •  » Communication Without Speech
  • Communication Without Speech

Communication Without Speech: Augmentative and Alternative Communication Around the World

By Anne Warrick (1998) ISAAC Series: Volume 1

Communication Without Speech

Communication Without Speech is an introduction to AAC. This book answers commonly asked questions and offers practical tips and illustrations. Topics include communication boards, vocabulary selection, assessment and education.

  • ISAAC Order Form (PDF, 118 kB)

Pre-payment required PDF downloadable file

Member price : Free to ISAAC Members (see Members Only section) Non-member price : $15 CDN

Communication Without Speech – Traditional Chinese (2010)

Communication without speech – simplified chinese (2010), comunicación sin habla: comunicación augmentativa y alternativa alrededor del mundo.

Escrito por: Anne Warrick (1998)

ISAAC Serie1

Comunicación Sin Habla

Comunicación Sin Habla es una excelente introduccion a la CAA; contiene gran cantidad de cuestiones y de herramientas practicas tales como seleccion de vocabulario, evaluacion, consideraciones educativas y profesionales, elaboracion de tableros, asi mismo incluye excelentes fotografias e ilustraciones.

Comunicacion Sin Habla

PDF downloadable file Pre-payment required

Precio de miembro : Free to ISAAC Members (see Members Only section) Precio de no miembro : $15 CDN

Print

  • Affiliated Publications
  • Beneath the Surface
  • Waves of Words
  • Conference Proceedings

Your support provides rehabilitation services to children from all over the United States.

Reading to your child.

How to Use Books to Promote Communication Skills

Whether or not your child has a disability, reading, and story-telling is a critical way to help them develop speech and language skills. 

We use books and story-telling with children to help promote brain development, teach language and emotions, spark curiosity, and encourage children to use their imagination. The book you choose to read is not as important as how you read it to your child. Some books have many words and some books have no words. Feel free to read the books and remember to interact with your child throughout the reading activity. 

You can do this by pointing out objects and characters, asking questions about what the characters are doing, describing pictures in the book, calling attention to colors, actions, emotions, animals, or people. This will help your child to develop a robust vocabulary as kids are visual and auditory learners.

Reading every word in the book is an option but not required to engage your child in a language-rich learning experience. Books that include songs, rhymes, and repetitions are a great way to get your child involved in the story. Have them repeat common phrases, sing-a-long, and predict rhymes throughout the story to keep them engaged. 

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • HHS Author Manuscripts

Logo of hhspa

Personalized Early AAC Intervention to Build Language and Literacy Skills: A Case Study of a 3-Year-Old with Complex Communication Needs

Personalized AAC intervention refers to an approach in which intervention is tailored to the individual’s needs and skills, the needs and priorities of the individual’s family and other social environments, the evidence base, and the individual’s response to intervention. This approach is especially relevant to AAC intervention for young children with complex communication needs given their unique constellations of strengths and challenges, and the qualitative and quantitative changes that they experience over time as they develop, as well as the diversity of their families, schools, and communities. This paper provides detailed documentation of personalized AAC intervention over a six-month period for a 3-year-old girl with developmental delay and complex communication needs. The paper describes (1) personalization of multimodal AAC supports to provide this child with the tools to communicate; (2) personalized intervention to build semantic and morphosyntactic skills; and, (3) personalized instruction in literacy skills (i.e., letter-sound correspondences, sound blending, decoding, sight word recognition, reading simple stories, reading comprehension, and encoding skills). Specific goals, instructional materials, and procedures are described; data on speech, language, and literacy outcomes are presented.

The terms, personalized medicine or personalized rehabilitation, refer to a model of service delivery in which interventions are tailored to the individual based on their unique make up, the evidence base, and the individual’s anticipated response to the intervention (e.g., Beukelman, 2016 ; Hamburg & Collins, 2010 ; Nonnekes & Nieuwboer, 2018 ). This personalized approach is focused on ensuring that individuals receive the right intervention at the right time (cf. Hamburg & Collins, 2010 ). This model stands in contrast to a “one size fits all” approach in which all individuals receive the same intervention regardless of their individual needs and skills or the characteristics of their environments ( Beukelman, 2016 ). A personalized approach is particularly relevant to guide decision-making in service delivery for young children who have complex communication needs (e.g., children with autism spectrum disorder, cerebral palsy, Down syndrome, or other developmental disabilities whose natural speech is not sufficient to meet all of their communication needs; Beukelman & Light, 2020 ). These children require augmentative and alternative communication (AAC) to support their communication. AAC is defined as strategies, techniques, and tools used to enhance communication ( ASHA, 2005 ). A vast array of AAC strategies, technologies, and interventions are available that may support children with complex communication needs and decisions must be made as to which of these are the most appropriate fit for each individual child ( Beukelman & Light, 2020 ).

Young children with complex communication needs each have unique constellations of strengths and challenges that impact their response to available AAC supports and interventions (e.g., Romski et al., 2015 ; Romski et al., 2010 ; Kasari et al., 2014 ). They experience significant changes as they learn and grow; their needs and skills show both quantitative and qualitative shifts over time, necessitating modifications to AAC strategies, technologies, and interventions over time to support their development ( Light & McNaughton, 2012 ). Furthermore, children with complex communication needs do not exist in isolation; rather they interact within dynamic social systems including their families, schools, and broader communities. These children and their families are also impacted by the broader societal context that defines legislation, policy, and practices (e.g., Beukelman & Light, 2020 ). Each of these social systems present their own unique constellations of strengths, challenges, values, and priorities that impact the uptake and ultimate success (or failure) of AAC systems and interventions ( Mandak et al., 2017 ). For these reasons, the most effective AAC interventions are personalized so that they take into account the child’s needs and skills; the needs and priorities of the family, school, and broader social context; and the available evidence base ( Granlund et al., 2008 ; Mandak et al., 2017 ). Moreover, the most effective AAC interventions are dynamic; the team carefully monitors the child’s response to intervention and makes modifications as required to reflect changes in the child’s needs and skills as well as those of the environment ( Beukelman & Light, 2020 ).

AAC interventions should support young children with complex communication needs so that they can participate actively in family, school, and community activities; these interventions should prepare them for later involvement in a wide range of meaningful adult activities (e.g., education, employment, volunteer activities, healthcare, and community living; McNaughton & Beukelman, 2010 ). To support such participation, personalized AAC interventions for children may need to target a broad range of individual goals to build communicative competence, foster language development, promote literacy learning, and enhance speech development ( Light & McNaughton, 2014 ). In order to maximize outcomes for young children with complex communication needs, it is critical for practitioners to understand how to personalize AAC supports and interventions to meet targeted goals under a variety of social and environmental conditions at different stages of development.

The field of AAC is still in the early stages of investigating the personalization of AAC intervention. Future research is required to understand how to best implement intervention in an individualized and evidence-based manner ( Beukelman, 2016 ). Detailed case studies are an important first step to identify factors that warrant further investigation. In fact, it has been argued that: “… a scientific discipline without a large number of thoroughly executed case studies is a discipline without systematic production of exemplars, and a discipline without exemplars is an ineffective one” ( Flyvbjerg, 2006 ; p. 219). This paper is intended to contribute to advancing our understanding by providing a comprehensive case study of personalized AAC intervention with a 3-year-old child with complex communication needs over a six-month period (from 38 to 44 months old). The personalized AAC intervention focused on two key domains across this period: (1) building language skills (specifically, semantic and morphosyntactic skills) to support more effective communication; and, (2) teaching literacy skills to facilitate generative communication. The paper provides detailed documentation of intervention goals, AAC supports, and instructional procedures across three phases of intervention over the six-month period and presents data on speech, language, and literacy outcomes. The case study illustrates how we implemented personalized, evidence-based AAC intervention and how we modified intervention based on the child’s response to intervention (RTI; Fuchs & Fuchs, 2006 ). It should be noted that this case study does not provide experimental control and therefore it is not possible to draw a definitive causal relationship between the AAC intervention and the outcomes observed; however, this case provides a source of data to help guide future intervention research.

Background Information

We first met Jessica 1 when she was 35 months old. She was an only child and lived at home with her parents who were very supportive and actively involved with her. When Jessica was 15 months old, her parents had requested an early intervention assessment due to concerns about delays in her acquisition of speech and motor developmental milestones. She qualified for early intervention services at that time and started to receive home-based speech and language intervention, occupational and physical therapy, and vision services. At 26 months, she had a neurodevelopmental evaluation and was diagnosed with global developmental delays with severe expressive language delay, mild receptive language delay, and neuromotor dysfunction. She also had a diagnosis of strabismus-intermittent esotropia (i.e., her eyes turned inward when fixating on objects or activities; Molarte et al., 1991 ) and rotational nystagmus (i.e., involuntary eye movement when the head is rotated; Tibbling, 1969 ). She had been followed by an ophthalmologist since she was 4 months of age and had successful surgery to correct her eye alignment when she was 41 months old (during the course of our intervention). There were no concerns about her hearing. She had passed her newborn hearing screening as well as more formal sound-field testing completed when she was a year old; she responded consistently to spoken input and environmental sounds.

At 35 months of age, Jessica received a comprehensive assessment of her motor, receptive language, expressive communication, and cognitive skills by her early intervention team in preparation for her transition to preschool services. At that time, she demonstrated the ability to walk independently, but did not run or jump. She was able to point with her index finger independent of her thumb and other fingers as well as scribble linear and circular patterns using a fist grasp. She required assistance to hold the paper while coloring, string beads, and fasten her clothing. Her cognitive development was assessed using the Battelle Developmental Inventory, Second Edition (BDI-2 ; Newborg, 2005 ). Her overall cognitive development domain score was 1.67 standard deviations below the mean, qualifying her for preschool services with the diagnosis of developmental delay. Her social and emotional development was also assessed using the BDI-2 . Her score fell within the average range for children with typical development. Per report, she greeted familiar adults spontaneously, enjoyed activities with adults, and responded positively when familiar adults initiated social contact. She separated easily from her parents, used adults for resources, and asked for help when needed. She was able to follow rules within the classroom. She was reported to engage in parallel play with peers and was observed to imitate the play activities of other children. She did not initiate social contact with peers; however, she willingly shared toys with them.

When she was 35 months old, her communication skills were also assessed using the Preschool Language Scale 5 th Edition (PLS-5 ; Zimmerman et al., 2011 ), a language sample, and a phonetic inventory. Her scores on the auditory comprehension portion of the PLS-5 were within the average range for children her age. She was reported to engage in symbolic play, recognize actions in photos, and understand quantitative concepts (e.g., one, some, all). Neither her parents nor her early intervention team reported concerns with Jessica’s receptive language abilities. Her speech production was extremely limited; standardized assessment tools were not administered due to her limited sound repertoire. Her phonetic inventory only included six consonant sounds. Her production of vowels was often distorted and her word shapes were reduplicated open syllables (CV, CVCV). Jessica presented with challenges in saliva management and was often observed to have her tongue out while engaging in activities.

Her expressive communication scores on the PLS-5 fell below the average range for children her age. She recognized photographs and line drawings, but demonstrated difficulty naming objects in photographs due to her expressive limitations. She responded appropriately to bids for social attention and demonstrated referential looking. Her parents and early intervention team had introduced her to signs and a low-tech communication book to augment her communication. She demonstrated a preference for using signs to communicate with her parents and teachers. Her parents and early intervention team reported that she knew approximately 150 signs, including signs for people, animals, actions, objects, descriptors, etc. Her signs were approximations of the required hand shapes, locations, orientations, and movements, but were typically understood by adults who were familiar with her sign inventory. She also had a communication book that contained pictures to support her communication with others, especially those who were unfamiliar with signs. She communicated in single signs (MLU =1.0) with a limited range of concepts. She used conventional gestures to communicate yes (head nod) and no (head shake). Jessica loved reading books with adults and her parents frequently read to her. Her parents had taught her the names of the letters of the alphabet, but not the sounds; she had not received any literacy instruction.

We used the Participation Model (as described in Beukelman & Light, 2020 ) to guide intervention, including consideration of Jessica’s participation patterns and communication needs, her capabilities and skills, and environmental supports and constraints (see Table 1 for a summary of each of these domains when Jessica was 35 months old).

Summary of Jessica’s communication needs and skills at 35 months of age, prior to the personalized AAC intervention.

Personalized AAC Intervention to Build Language and Literacy Skills

When Jessica turned three years old, she transitioned to a community preschool program three half days per week where she received speech and language therapy, physical therapy, and vision services. She was also referred to the Penn State AAC program where she attended an early language group two mornings per week followed by individual instruction for 30 minutes (the focus of this paper); she started these programs at the age of 38 months. Based on the assessment of her needs and skills and environmental supports and constraints, the following areas were identified as priorities for intervention in collaboration with her family and preschool team: (1) enhancing social communication skills with her peers; (2) building her language skills (with a focus on semantic and morphosyntactic skills) to allow her to share her experiences and participate effectively in education; and, (3) learning literacy skills to support generative communication and learning. The first of these skills was addressed during the early language group and is not explicitly discussed in this paper. This paper focuses primarily on her individual sessions that provided literacy instruction with opportunities to enhance language skills integrated into the literacy instruction. The language and literacy intervention was delivered by the third author for the first three months and the fourth author for the following three months, both graduate students in speech language pathology; these students were supervised by a qualified speech language pathologist with expertise in AAC (the second author) while the first author provided consultative support.

As noted earlier, intervention occurred across a six-month period and included three phases, each building on the skills acquired in the prior phase. Data were collected regularly to allow formative evaluation of her performance; modifications to goals, instructional materials, and procedures were made regularly depending on her performance. Thus, intervention was dynamic, reflecting her response to instruction. Table 2 presents a summary of the three intervention phases as well as the language and literacy goals targeted in each phase.

Phases of intervention and targeted language and literacy goals across a six-month period.

Jessica’s parents were actively involved in intervention planning; a parent typically observed the intervention sessions and followed through with many activities in the home environment. Her parents regularly used the following interaction strategies with Jessica to support her communication: providing numerous opportunities for her to communicate, responding to her communication attempts, and expanding on her messages using speech plus signs. They read books regularly with her and talked about the stories, relating them to her experiences.

AAC Supports for Communication

Too often, AAC supports are selected for young children based on clinician preference or the practices of the educational program. In contrast, in keeping with the principles of personalized intervention, we selected and customized AAC supports for Jessica based on her needs, skills, and preferences as well as the values and preferences of her family and preschool program. Most individuals with complex communication needs rely on multiple modes to communicate (e.g., Light et al., 1985 ). Thus, we supported Jessica in the use of multiple modes to enhance her communication across partners and environments, including use of natural speech; gestures and signs; low tech photos, pictures, written words, and letter cards; and mobile technology with an AAC app and keyboard.

The research clearly demonstrates that AAC intervention does not inhibit speech production in individuals with complex communication needs; in fact, the evidence suggests that AAC enhances speech production (e.g., Millar et al., 2006 ; Kasari et al., 2014 ; Romski & Sevcik, 2005 ; Romski et al., 2010 ). Furthermore, based on preliminary observational research of children with complex communication needs, reported by Light and McNaughton (2017) , we believed that literacy learning might be especially facilitative of speech production as the written word would provide a visual support for production of the target spoken word. Thus, we did not consider it to be an “either-or” decision to target natural speech or AAC; rather, as recommended by Oommen and McCarthy (2015) , we worked with Jessica to further her speech development and also provided her with access to AAC (with an emphasis on literacy) to augment her speech and foster her language development. We modeled speech and AAC at all times and responded to all of her speech attempts. Measures of her speech production were collected regularly to determine gains.

Jessica had been introduced to signs during her home-based early intervention program as an infant and toddler; her parents frequently watched the Signing Time videos 2 and signed regularly with her. We continued to emphasize the use of signs with Jessica for a variety of reasons. First and foremost, the research demonstrates the importance of considering preference in selecting AAC supports for individuals with complex communication needs (e.g., van der Meer et al., 2011 ). Consideration of preferences plays a critical role in self-determination and uptake of AAC ( Sigafoos et al., 2005 ). Jessica showed a preference for signs over aided AAC (e.g., low tech photos or pictures; AAC technology). In addition, signs were readily accessible throughout her day and they were easily integrated into all activities (e.g., play, meals, bath, book reading). Furthermore, Jessica was able to approximate the hand shape and movement of the signs without undue effort. She was able to acquire new vocabulary spontaneously by watching others sign throughout her day, using fast mapping to acquire new vocabulary concepts as she needed them (just as children with typical development acquire new spoken words). In addition, Jessica’s parents preferred the spontaneity and accessibility of signs over aided AAC technology; they had spent time developing their own signing skills to support her communication and made an effort to sign with her throughout the day. Jessica and her family had already demonstrated significant uptake and use of signs to enhance her communication. Thus, we built on these individual and family strengths and preferences, encouraging her service providers to sign with her at all times thus modeling language use via speech and sign.

Despite the significant advantages of manual signs as a means to enhance Jessica’s communication, it was clear that this modality was not effective to meet her needs when communicating with peers or adults who were unfamiliar with signs. Therefore, we worked with Jessica and her family to introduce mobile technology with an AAC app to support her communication, especially in the early language group and her preschool program. In keeping with personalized AAC intervention, we reviewed her communication needs and skills and environmental supports and constraints as outlined in Table 1 . As recommended as part of the AAC assessment-intervention process ( Binger et al., 2012 ), based on these factors, we identified the features that Jessica required and selected potential apps that incorporated these features. We then provided opportunities for trial use within her preschool language group and collected data on her accuracy, the effectiveness of her communication, her efficiency, and her preferences. Based on these data, with input from her family and service providers, we introduced her to TouchChat with WordPower 3 42 Basic on a 10.2-inch iPad. The WordPower vocabulary file was used as a foundation; personal vocabulary was added for Jessica as needed (e.g. personal information, family members, preferred activities, classroom themes, etc.). TouchChat with WordPower provided Jessica with access to a range of vocabulary concepts, including a home page of frequently used concepts and 45 main categories of fringe vocabulary plus additional subcategories). Within the WordPower vocabulary file, Jessica had access to numerous displays reflecting various activities (e.g. art, calendar time). The vocabulary concepts were represented by line drawings (i.e. Symbolstix), organized in a grid layout of rows and columns. Each display was organized using a semantic-syntactic organization (see Beukelman & Light, 2020 ) to promote the development of sentence structures. Instruction with the application focused on (1) using a robust vocabulary of semantic concepts (e.g., people, animals, actions, colors, etc.) to express a wide range of messages to fulfill a breadth of communicative functions; and (2) supporting Jessica to communicate phrases and sentences around her areas of interest. For example, when engaging in a classroom activity focused on sports, Jessica used the app to spontaneously communicate, “I like basketball.” As Jessica acquired literacy skills during intervention, she also made use of the TouchChat WordPower phonetic keyboard to type words (see the section on literacy instruction for more details).

We also provided Jessica with access to low tech AAC supports (e.g., photos, line drawings, written words, letter cards) to allow her to participate fully in educational activities and literacy learning and to support communication when manual signs and her AAC technology did not meet her needs. Thus, Jessica had access to multimodal AAC (including natural speech, gestures and signs, low tech AAC, and mobile technology with an AAC app) to enhance her communication and increase her participation across partners and environments. Although these AAC supports were important, simply providing access to these tools was not sufficient to ensure the development of communicative competence ( Light & McNaughton, 2014 ); rather Jessica also needed to develop language and literacy skills to enhance her communication.

Intervention to Support Language Development

When Jessica started to receive AAC services at Penn State when she was 38 months old, she already demonstrated well developed pragmatic skills with familiar adults (e.g., she fulfilled her turns in interactions, maintained the topic, and responded contingently). Therefore, language intervention focused on supporting peer interactions in the early language group, and on fostering semantic and morphosyntactic development to allow her to communicate more complex messages about a wide range of topics and to prepare her to be a full participant in her educational program. In general, we used what is known about the stages of early language development to guide our intervention goals and procedures (see Gerber & Kraat, 1992 ; Beukelman & Light, 2020 ). When Jessica was referred, she was at the stage of early symbolic development. She was an intentional communicator and had acquired more than 100 signs as well as a small number of spoken word approximations; however, she was not yet combining concepts to communicate more complex messages. At this stage, priorities were to continue to build her vocabulary and to extend the complexity of her messages (see Table 2 ).

Language intervention followed a social pragmatic approach, driven by Jessica’s interests, needs, and skills. Some individuals with complex communication needs benefit from explicit instruction to support vocabulary learning ( Reichle & Drager, 2010 ; Wilkinson & McIlvane, 2002 ). However, Jessica demonstrated the ability to use fast-mapping to acquire new language concepts in the context of her daily activities so we built on this individual strength and focused on this approach to intervention, supporting her language development in response to her needs and interests in naturally occurring interactions. Fast mapping is a developmental phenomenon whereby children acquire an initial understanding of a word’s meaning with only minimal exposure; this initial understanding is subsequently fine-tuned with ongoing exposure and use of the word ( Wilkinson & Albert, 2001 ). Throughout the early language group and her individual intervention sessions, Jessica was provided with numerous opportunities to acquire new vocabulary concepts and combine concepts using the following evidence-based strategies: (1) setting up meaningful opportunities for communication; (2) waiting to provide Jessica with the time to express herself; (3) responding to all of her communicative attempts by fulfilling her intent (e.g., requesting information, commenting on experiences, requesting activities); (4) expanding on her message to model more complex language; and, (5) using speech plus AAC (e.g., speech plus signs, speech plus written or typed text) in interactions with her to provide language models that were appropriate given her comprehension skills. The research suggests: (1) the importance of partner responsivity to foster communication early in development (e.g., Warren & Brady, 2007 ); (2) the positive impact of partner models on the expression of longer, more complex messages (e.g., Binger et al., 2008 ; Binger & Light, 2007 ; Binger et al., 2011 ); and (3) the facilitative effect of AAC input from partners on all aspects of language development, including pragmatic, semantic, and morphosyntactic development (e.g., Allen et al., 2017 ; O’Neill et al., 2018 ). Furthermore, the research suggests that children with complex communication needs are frequently pre-empted from opportunities to communicate by partners who dominate the interaction; waiting or expectant delay is a powerful strategy to cue children with complex communication needs to take a turn and to provide them with the time to do so ( Kent-Walsh et al., 2015 ).

We provided Jessica with exposure to a rich language learning environment using speech and AAC to model a wide range of concepts reflecting various semantic roles. In recent years, some researchers have recommended a “universal core vocabulary” approach for beginning communicators (e.g., Geist & Erickson, 2016 ; Project Core, 2020 ); this approach focuses on teaching a very small number of words to beginning communicators. Despite the popularity of the core vocabulary approach with some service providers, we did not restrict Jessica’s vocabulary or specifically emphasize a small number of pre-determined words. There is no empirical evidence to support the effectiveness of this type of core vocabulary approach with beginning communicators as a foundation for robust language learning ( Laubscher & Light, 2020 ). In fact, research suggests that development of a large, diverse vocabulary is a strong predictor of later language and literacy outcomes (e.g., Hemphill & Tivnan, 2008 ; Dickinson et al., 2003 ). Very few of the words included in core vocabulary lists actually reflect the kinds of words that children learn early in language development at the first words stage (cf. Fenson et al., 1994 ). Furthermore, there is no evidence to support restricting expressive vocabulary to a limited set of “core” words; rather, children’s ability to produce word combinations relies heavily on their acquisition of a robust vocabulary including a range of semantic concepts ( Fenson et al., 1994 ). Children with typical development only start to combine language concepts when they have acquired a sufficient number of concepts to communicate a range of semantic relations, that is, once they have acquired approximately 50 concepts ( Laubscher & Light, 2020 ). Thus, vocabulary development is critical to later morphosyntactic development (e.g., Marchman & Bates, 1994 ).

For these reasons, we immersed Jessica in a rich language learning environment that followed principles of language development and regularly introduced new, developmentally-appropriate concepts in response to her interests and environmental demands (e.g., themes in her preschool, concepts from her favorite books). We strove to provide AAC models of everything that was said to her so that she could hear and see language used in communication. Throughout the personalized intervention, language targets were continually revised to reflect Jessica’s learning and her changing needs and skills; in other words, we kept “raising the bar,” targeting more and more complex language concepts and structures.

As summarized in Table 2 , in Phase 1 of the intervention, we started by targeting acquisition of a wide range of single vocabulary concepts representing a variety of semantic roles (e.g., agents, actions, objects, descriptors, locatives, question words, social words) and expression of early emerging two-word combinations (e.g., semantic relations such as agent-action, action-object, descriptor-object; Phase 1). Once Jessica was regularly combining two concepts via speech, signs, and/or aided AAC, we continued to expand her vocabulary introducing new concepts every day in response to her needs and skills; and we used expectant delay to mark opportunities and provide time for her to express longer and more complex sentence structures (Phase 2).

As Jessica built greater fluency combining concepts, we gradually introduced specific function words and morphological and syntactic structures (e.g., use of articles, auxiliaries, conjunctions, etc.) in Phase 3 of intervention. We did not target these concepts in isolation; rather we introduced them in the context of discourse and narratives. As Boenisch and Soto (2015) noted, “…function words become meaningful when used in combination with other words and are typically acquired through discourse” (p. 82). This third phase of language intervention coincided with Jessica’s transition to reading sentences and stories (see the discussion of literacy intervention in the following section). Thus, in addition to modeling language using signs plus speech, we also modeled more complex sentence structures using written text. These models using written language were particularly powerful as they provided a permanent visual support for Jessica to learn specific aspects of morphology (e.g., third person singular -s; articles a or the). The written models were also important at this stage as Jessica’s need for more complex input was beginning to outstrip the sign language skills of her communication partners. Jessica used many of these morphosyntactic structures first when reading and later in her spontaneous interactions.

Language Intervention Outcomes

We collected data on Jessica’s speech and language outcomes using the following techniques: (1) parent and clinician report of vocabulary concepts expressed spontaneously; and (2) transcription and coding of language samples collected each week from informal interactions at the beginning and end of sessions. We analyzed her expression via multiple modalities including natural speech, manual signs, and aided AAC. Table 3 presents data on the language outcomes of intervention. As illustrated in the table, Jessica demonstrated substantial gains in the frequency of her communication turns over the six-month intervention, increasing from a rate of approximately 5–6 turns per minute at baseline to a rate of 10 turns per minute. She also demonstrated substantial gains in the acquisition of vocabulary concepts across the six-month period, acquiring new concepts every day. In addition, she increased her mean length of message from 1.04 (e.g., a single telegraphic sign and/or speech approximation) to multiword sentences.

Language and speech production outcomes of intervention over a six-month period based on 10-min samples at baseline and after 3 and 6 months of intervention.

As is apparent from the examples in Table 3 , her expressive language development across the six-month intervention followed the sequence of typical language development: First she communicated via single words, typically content words (e.g., baby, bubble, mama); next she began to combine semantic concepts such as object + descriptor (baby sad) or action + object (open book); later she started to use longer sentence structures and began to incorporate some structural words including, for example, the use of conjunctions or articles (e.g., Dad has a blue coat). It is interesting to note that very few of the words used spontaneously by Jessica in her early communication messages were on the list of core vocabulary proposed for beginning communicators (e.g., Geist & Erickson, 2016 ; Project Core, 2020 ); rather she utilized a rich array of content words to express herself early in her language development. It was only later in her language development that she began to draw on some of the structural words that are more frequently occurring. As noted earlier, Jessica’s gains in morphosyntax were especially evident as she transitioned to reading sentences and stories where the written text provided a visual model of the structures.

Throughout intervention, Jessica relied on multiple modes to communicate using a combination of signs and natural speech in spontaneous interactions, and also incorporating aided AAC during more structured interactions in the classroom. For example, when she was leaving to go home one day, Jessica communicated the following message about her backpack (which had a picture of a fox, one of her favorite animals): “Fox backpack there” with the word, “fox”, communicated via sign alone, and the words, “backpack there,” communicated via natural speech and sign simultaneously. At the end of a session, she pointed out her dad’s coat and spontaneously communicated, “Dad has a blue coat” using speech and sign simultaneously to express each word. As noted earlier, Jessica also used aided AAC to express herself in more structured interactions. In a discussion of sports in the early language group, she used her AAC app to communicate: I like basketball. (See Table 3 for additional examples.)

Speech Production Outcomes

As noted earlier, throughout intervention, we modeled the use of both speech and AAC (sign and/or aided AAC) in interaction with Jessica and we responded to her communication attempts via any modality. As she developed literacy skills (see the section on literacy intervention for further details), the written text seemed to help to promote speech production. She knew the sounds produced by each letter and the written words provided a visual support marking each sound that should be produced in sequence. Her acquisition of the letters and sounds that underlie literacy coincided with significant increases in the number and range of consonants that she produced. In turn these increases resulted in substantial increases in the intelligibility of her spoken utterances to familiar partners in context. A speech sample at the start of intervention indicated that her phonetic inventory included six consonant sounds /b, p, m, w, d, g/. Her production of vowels was often distorted and her word shapes were reduplicated open syllables (CV, CVCV). In a 10-min sample of interaction, she produced a total of 63 consonants, but 76% of these were either /m/ or /b/. Only 33% of her spoken utterances were intelligible in context (i.e., number of intelligible words out of number of total words). After six-months of language and literacy intervention (when she was 44 months old), she demonstrated significant gains in her speech production. She used a total of 139 consonants in a 10-min sample, including 12 different consonants (only 28% of these were /m/ or /b/ demonstrating much greater diversity of speech sounds). With her increased range of consonants, the intelligibility of her spoken utterances also improved with 95% intelligible for familiar partners in context.

Intervention to Support Literacy Learning

Although language intervention was primarily driven by principles of typical development in determining appropriate goals, we “tampered” with the model of typical development (cf. Gerber & Kraat, 1992 ) and introduced instruction in conventional literacy skills earlier than might usually be expected. We considered the research evidence and Jessica’s and her family’s strengths and priorities in making this decision. The decision was driven by the following considerations. First, literacy skills are of significant importance in society and are fundamental to participation in all aspects of daily life, including education, employment, social interaction, activities of daily living, healthcare, and community living ( Machalicek et al., 2010 ). Literacy skills take on even greater importance for individuals with complex communication needs who have limited speech as they provide access to generative communication. Without literacy skills, children with complex communication needs remain dependent on others to provide and teach picture symbols to represent various vocabulary concepts. With literacy skills, they are able to independently generate any message to express themselves ( Light & McNaughton, 2020 ). Therefore, the development of literacy skills offered Jessica tremendous potential to enhance her communication and support her full participation in educational activities.

In addition, literacy provides a powerful support for language learning. Through literacy, children with complex communication needs have access to a rich vocabulary of concepts (including ones that might not typically be encountered in daily activities), thus furthering their semantic and cognitive development ( Clendon et al., 2014 ). In addition, as noted earlier, written text provides a powerful visual support for learning the morphosyntactic aspects of language. Morphosyntactic skills are especially vulnerable in children with complex communication needs. The research shows that children who rely on AAC typically communicate in face-to-face interactions using telegraphic forms (e.g., Light et al., 1985 ). They often co-construct messages with their communication partners with a focus on the expression of meaning and communicative intent with limited attention to the structural aspects of language. In contrast, written language provides a clear visual representation of the structural aspects of language. Light and McNaughton (2017) have argued that the visual support provided by written text can be leveraged to support morphosyntactic learning by children with complex communication needs. Furthermore, literacy skills may also be beneficial for children with complex communication needs who experience significant challenges in speech production as written language makes explicit the individual speech sounds and their sequence in words through application of letter sound knowledge to the orthographic sequence of letters ( Light & McNaughton, 2017 ). Finally, literacy skills were a priority for Jessica’s parents. These skills are highly valued within society and the acquisition of literacy skills may significantly impact others’ perceptions of a child’s competence, thereby positively impacting learning opportunities and experiences ( Stanovich, 2008 ).

Overall, our approach to literacy instruction followed the evidence-based principles laid out by Light and McNaughton (2009) with the first phase of instruction targeting the foundations for literacy skills (i.e., basic skills such as letter sound correspondences and sound blending) as well as sight word instruction and shared reading to demonstrate the power of literacy (see Table 2 ). As soon as Jessica acquired approximately 6–7 letter sound correspondences, she transitioned to Phase 2 of instruction and we introduced her to decoding (i.e., the skills to look at the letters in a word in sequence, recall their sounds, and blend the sounds together to read the target word). We also continued instruction in the remaining letter sound correspondences, shared reading, and sight word recognition (specifically of frequently-occurring, irregular words and high interest complex words). Once Jessica was consistently applying decoding skills and once she had learned to recognize a corpus of frequently occurring sight words, we focused on teaching her to read and understand simple stories. We also introduced her to keyboarding skills, phoneme segmentation skills, and early encoding /writing skills (Phase 3).

Overall literacy intervention integrated direct instruction in basic skills (e.g., letter sound correspondences, phonological awareness skills, decoding, and encoding skills) with numerous opportunities to apply these skills in meaningful literacy experiences such as shared book reading and shared story writing ( Light & McNaughton, 2020 ). Direct instruction followed the procedures described by Light and McNaughton (2009) : (1) we modeled the target skill; (2) we provided guided practice in the skill (i.e., provided prompting support to ensure Jessica’s success); and (3) we provided independent practice in the skill with feedback for each response. When Jessica successfully demonstrated the target skill, we provided positive encouragement. When she demonstrated an incorrect response, we provided corrective feedback, modeling the correct response for her and providing her with guided practice in the skill. Over time, instructional supports were faded based on Jessica’s learning.

Key instructional materials were personalized to reflect Jessica’s interests and experiences. This personalization was critical to ensure her motivation and to support her understanding based on her experiences and world knowledge. For example, target words and books were personalized to include her family, her daily experiences, and her interest in animals. Instructional tasks and procedures were also adapted to accommodate Jessica’s needs. Specifically, as described by Light and McNaughton (2009) , we adapted instruction to allow her to participate using a range of modalities (e.g., natural speech; signs; photos, line drawings, written words, or letter cards; AAC technology). We provided support initially to help her re-auditorize written letters or words to speech (i.e., translating the letters, c-a-t, to the spoken word). The following sections describe these adaptations in more detail for each of the literacy skills targeted as well as the outcomes of instruction.

Letter-Sound Correspondences

At baseline, prior to instruction, Jessica knew the names of many letters but she did not consistently identify any of their sounds. Since it is knowledge of the sounds (not the names) of letters that is required for reading and writing, we focused on teaching letter sound correspondences, introducing letters and their sounds one at a time, until she acquired all of the individual letter sounds. Digraphs (e.g., th, sh) are typically introduced later in instruction. We started by teaching lower case rather than upper case letters as the vast majority of words in books include lower, not upper case letters. We taught upper case as appropriate in context (e.g., the first letter of her name or other proper names). We did not teach the letter sounds in alphabetical order; rather we taught them in the sequence recommended by Light and McNaughton (2009) , teaching those letter sounds that are most frequently occurring in children’s books first so that Jessica would be able to read more words earlier. When introducing new letter sounds, we separated ones that were visually and auditorily similar (e.g., m and n). Since Jessica was not able to reliably say the letter sounds, we adapted instruction to support her participation. Specifically, we said the letter sound and Jessica found the appropriate letter from a field of 4 or more letter cards ( Light & McNaughton, 2009 ). Once Jessica had learned to consistently identify 15 letters when presented with their sounds, we introduced her to a hard copy keyboard initially (and later a keyboard on the iPad) so that she could practice locating the letters on the keyboard for spelling and writing tasks. We used a standard QWERTY keyboard since Jessica had sufficient motor skills to select with an index finger; this keyboard is most frequently used in society and learning this keyboard would provide her with greatest flexibility across educational, vocational, and community environments.

Figure 1 presents data on Jessica’s acquisition of letter sound correspondences during the first two phases of intervention (Months 1–3). As the graph illustrates, she required a total of 22 instructional sessions (approximately 8 minutes each) to learn all letters and sounds (a total of approximately 170 minutes of instruction). Initially, she required several sessions to learn a single letter sound correspondence (e.g., 6 sessions to learn a; 2 sessions to learn m; 3 sessions to learn t). Once she had learned the first three letter sound correspondences, her rate of learning accelerated rapidly so that we consistently introduced two new letters each session.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms-1699201-f0001.jpg

Number of letter sound correspondences successfully acquired with >80% accuracy across baseline and intervention (Months 1–3 of intervention; 38–41 months old)

Sound Blending

Learning to read depends not just on letter sound knowledge but also on the phonological awareness skill of sound blending (i.e., blending sounds produced slowly to form words). Since Jessica was not able to reliably blend sounds and say words orally, we adapted instruction to support her participation. We said the letter sounds of a word slowly in sequence and then Jessica had to blend the sounds together in her head, determine the word, and then sign the word or indicate the photo of the target word from a field of 4 or more photos ( Light & McNaughton, 2009 ). At baseline, prior to intervention, Jessica performed at chance levels; she had not yet learned to hold the sounds in her working memory and blend them together to determine the target word. For the most part, she demonstrated steady gains and required a total of 10 instructional sessions during Months 1 and 2 of intervention (approximately 80 minutes total; 8 minutes for each session) to learn this phonological awareness skill (see Figure 2 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms-1699201-f0002.jpg

Percent accuracy with sound blending across baseline and intervention (Months 1–2 of intervention; 38–40 months old)

Sight Word Recognition and Shared Reading

In order to build motivation and ensure that Jessica recognized the power of literacy learning from the outset, we introduced her to approximately 12 high interest words early on which she learned to recognize by sight (before she learned to decode). These high interest words included: family members (i.e., her name, mom, dad), animals which were of great interest to her (i.e., cat, dog, fox, bear), and favorite activities (i.e., book, bike, cook, swing, rock). As with letter-sound instruction, we introduced these words to Jessica one at a time, over time. Once she had learned to recognize these words by sight, we introduced her to additional high interest sight words (e.g., sign, ball, jump, cookie, bubbles, snack, puzzle, juice, duck, cup).

We incorporated these words into simple books so that Jessica could participate in shared reading activities. Each page of the book included a photo of interest to her with a simple sentence typed under the photo; the high interest sight words were highlighted as they occurred. Initially, only a single word was highlighted on each page (often at the end of the sentence). Over time, as Jessica increased her sight word recognition skills and learned to decode words, additional words were targeted in each sentence (e.g., Jessica likes to cook with dad). A familiar adult (e.g., graduate student, parent) read the sentence to Jessica pausing at each of the highlighted words and waiting for Jessica to read the word and then sign it, say it, or point to a picture of the concept. These shared reading activities were her favorite instructional activities; they clearly demonstrated the power and joy of literacy learning.

Decoding Skills

As soon as Jessica had acquired 6–7 letter sounds and was able to reliably blend sounds, we taught her to decode regular words (e.g., CVC, VC, CV words). Decoding skills are essential to literacy learning for they allow children to attack new words that they have not been taught ( Ahlgrim-Delzell et al., 2016 ; Fallon et al., 2004 ). It is challenging to learn to decode. The child must look at the letters in sequence, recall the sound for each letter, hold these sounds in working memory, and then blend them together to determine the word. As Jessica acquired new letters and sounds, new words that included these letter sounds were regularly added to decoding instruction. Thus the task became more challenging over time.

Decoding instruction followed the adapted procedures described by Light and McNaughton (2009) : We provided Jessica with four photos or pictures, each representing a different CVC word; we then provided her with the written word; initially we provided her with guided practice and helped her to point to each of the letters in sequence and say the letter sound; she then blended the sounds together and selected the photo that represented the word. Over time, we faded our support so that Jessica was expected to look at the letters in sequence, retrieve their sounds in her head, and blend them together independently (see Light & McNaughton, 2009 for further details). We selected the foils carefully so that, for the most part, each of the words represented by the photos differed from the target by a single letter-sound (e.g., if the target word was bug, the foils might be rug, bag, and bun); thus, we ensured that Jessica was decoding the entire word and not relying on recognizing the first letter(s). As noted by Mandak et al. (2018) , there are various adaptations that can be used to support the participation of children with complex communication needs in literacy instruction (e.g., saying the target word and asking the child to choose from a field of four or more written words; providing a written word and asking the child to choose the picture that the word represents from a field of four or more, etc.). We specifically chose an approach that required Jessica to re-code the word phonologically from its written form since this is a process that is typically challenging for children with complex communication needs (e.g., Dahlgren Sandberg, 2002 ). As Jessica learned to decode words, regular decodable words were also targeted in her shared reading books along with the sight words that she had acquired.

Figure 3 presents data on Jessica’s performance independently decoding single words. At baseline, as expected with most 3-year-olds, Jessica was not able to decode words. She required 25 instructional sessions from Months 2–6 (a total of approximately 300 minutes of instruction, approximately 10–12 minutes of instruction per session) to learn to decode single words and apply these word attack skills to new words. Her performance demonstrated some variability from session to session since she was expected to decode words including a steadily increasing range of letters and sounds over time.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms-1699201-f0003.jpg

Percent accuracy independently decoding single words during baseline and intervention (Months 2–6; and then during tele-practice after a 2-month break due to COVID 19, marked by the third phase change line). Note that the number of letter-sounds and words targeted increased significantly across sessions as Jessica acquired new letter sounds and was able to read new words (marked by the second phase change line).

Reading Simple Stories

As Jessica acquired decoding skills and learned to recognize many irregular frequently-occurring words by sight, she began to take more and more responsibility in the shared reading activities until she was reading full sentences and simple stories independently. Figure 4 presents data on Jessica’s performance reading simple stories (that were new to her) across 10 in-person instructional sessions conducted during Months 5–6, followed by a two-month break in instruction due to the COVID-19 pandemic and then subsequently followed by six additional telepractice sessions across a one-month period. As is evident in the graph, the percentage of target words that she read independently increased steadily over time until she was consistently reading novel simple stories with greater than 80% accuracy.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms-1699201-f0004.jpg

Percent accuracy reading words in sentences and simple stories during intervention (Months 5–6) and then during tele-practice after a 2-month break due to COVID 19 (marked by the phase change line)

Early Writing Skills

Literacy skills involve not just reading, but also writing activities. In this paper, we use the term writing to refer to the production of written text via whatever means (e.g., sequencing letter cards, typing). Learning to write is more difficult that learning to read as the writing process places greater demands on working memory skills (e.g., Ehri, 2000 ). In addition, writing requires coordination of a writing implement of some sort (e.g., pencil, marker, letter cards, keyboard; e.g., Millar et al., 2004 ). From the start of intervention, we ensured that Jessica had access to writing /typing tools and that she saw models of others writing and typing, but we waited to start instruction in conventional writing skills until she had demonstrated basic reading skills. Once Jessica had acquired 15 letter sounds, we introduced Jessica to instruction to start to build conventional writing skills (see Table 2 ). We started by introducing her to the keyboard. As noted earlier, we introduced her to the QWERTY keyboard on her iPad as she had the motor and visual capabilities to use this layout; furthermore, it is the standard used in the vast majority of technology and it would provide maximum flexibility later in her educational program. We used the TouchChat WordPower phonetic keyboard which had lower case letters that produced the letter sounds (rather than the letter names) upon selection. Jessica rapidly learned to locate and select the letters from the keyboard when presented with the sounds; her accuracy increased from 0% in session 1 to >90% after 14 sessions conducted over a three-month period during Months 3–6 (a total of approximately 37 minutes of instruction).

In Phase 3, we also worked with Jessica to introduce her to early encoding skills. We encouraged Jessica to choose a photo or picture of interest to her and then write a story by first determining the word that she wanted to write, breaking it down into component sounds, identifying the letters to represent each of the component sounds in sequence, and then selecting the appropriate letter from a group of letter cards or from the keyboard). This instruction was ongoing when sessions had to be terminated due to the COVID-19 pandemic.

Conclusions

This case study illustrates the implementation of personalized AAC intervention targeting language and literacy skills to support a 3-year-old child diagnosed with developmental delay and complex communication needs. Prior to intervention, she demonstrated strengths in her pragmatic skills with familiar adults, but she had significant limitations in her expressive vocabulary and her morphosyntactic skills. She communicated using a range of single signs and some speech approximations (approximately 33% intelligible in context). She enjoyed books and had learned the names for the letters, but did not know the letter sounds and was not able to decode, read, or produce written text. She demonstrated substantial gains in language, speech, and literacy skills following personalized AAC intervention that was driven by the research evidence and Jessica’s and her family’s needs, strengths, and priorities. Critical to these gains was the focus on: (1) setting challenging goals; (2) providing effective instructional supports to attain these goals; and (3) carefully monitoring her response to intervention. We collected data on her performance throughout intervention and modified instructional goals, materials, and procedures as required. It is especially important to note how rapidly we revised goals based on her response to intervention. Specifically, we regularly “raised the bar,” setting appropriately high expectations and targeting progressively more complex skills as she demonstrated gains in each domain. Too often, expectations for young children with complex communication needs are too low and goals are set for extended time periods. The personalized approach to intervention implemented in this case ensured that we maximized gains during the intervention period by carefully monitoring response to intervention and adjusting accordingly.

After six months of intervention, Jessica had acquired a wide range of semantic concepts in response to her interests and needs; she regularly combined these concepts to communicate multi-word messages and demonstrated increased use of the structural components of language (e.g., articles, conjunctions, etc.). Her speech production of vowels improved and the number and diversity of consonants that she produced also showed significant improvements; her spoken messages were 95% intelligible to familiar adults in context. After six months of intervention, at the age of 3 years 8 months, her literacy skills exceeded those of most of her peers with typical development significantly. She knew all of her letter sounds, demonstrated phonological awareness skills (e.g., sound blending), decoded and recognized by sight a wide range of single words, read simple sentences and stories, located letters accurately on a keyboard, and was beginning to demonstrate encoding /spelling skills. Jessica’s parents reported that they had observed similar gains in her performance during daily activities at home. She had established a solid foundation of language and literacy skills to prepare her to participate actively in education, family, and community activities.

Despite the positive results of this study, it is important to note that the case study design does not provide experimental control so it is not possible to draw definite conclusions regarding the effects of the intervention. Furthermore, outcomes were measured primarily through performance within individual sessions, supplemented by the report of her parents and intervention team. Future research is required to systematically investigate the effects of personalized early AAC intervention on the language and literacy skills of a larger number of young children with complex communication needs in order to establish intervention effectiveness. This future research should target measures of speech, language, and literacy performance not only within clinical sessions, but also within daily activities in the natural environment to determine the real-world effects. Such research is critical to ensure young children with complex communication needs receive the effective personalized AAC intervention they require to develop language and literacy skills and attain communicative competence.

Acknowledgments

The contents of this paper were developed under grants to the Rehabilitation Engineering Research Center on Augmentative and Alternative Communication (The RERC on AAC) from the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, National Institute on Disability, Independent Living, and Rehabilitation Research (NIDILRR): grant # 90RE5017 (2014-20) and grant #90REGE0014-01-00 (2020-25). The third author was supported through a personnel preparation grant funded by the Office of Special Education Programs of the U.S. Department of Education (grant #H325K170130). The contents do not necessarily represent the policy of the funding agencies, and endorsement by the federal government should not be assumed. We also wish to gratefully acknowledge the funding received from the Hintz Family Endowment to support the AAC program at Penn State University. Most importantly, we wish to thank “Jessica” and her parents for allowing us to be a part of their lives and for graciously consenting to share their experiences.

1. This paper uses a pseudonym to protect confidentiality.

2. Signing Time resources are available from Two Little Hands Productions at https://www.signingtime.com/

3. TouchChat with WordPower is manufactured by PRC – Saltillo, 1022 Heyl Road, Wooster, OH 44691; https://www.prentrom.com/

  • Ahlgrim-Delzell L, Browder DM, Wood L, Stanger C, Preston AI, & Kemp-Inman A (2016). Systematic instruction of phonics skills using an iPad for students with developmental disabilities who are AAC users . The Journal of Special Education , 50 , 86–97. doi: 10.1177/0022466915622140 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Allen AA, Schlosser RW, Brock KL, & Shane C (2017). The effectiveness of aided augmented input techniques for persons with developmental disabilities: A systematic review . Augmentative and Alternative Communication , 33 , 149–159. doi: 10.1080/07434618.2017.1338752 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • American Speech Language Hearing Association (ASHA) (2005). Roles and responsibilities of speech language pathologists with respect to augmentative and alternative communication: Position statement . doi: 10.1044/policy.PS2005-00113 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Beukelman DR (2016). Precision intervention research for adults with complex communication needs resulting from acquired conditions . Augmentative and Alternative Communication , 32 , 233–240. doi: 10.1080/07434618.2016.1252947 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Beukelman DR & Light J (2020). Augmentative and alternative communication: Supporting children and adults with complex communication needs . Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Binger C, Ball L, Dietz A, Kent-Walsh J, Lasker J, Lund S, McKelvey M, & Quach W (2012). Personnel roles in the AAC assessment process . Augmentative and Alternative Communication , 28 , 278–288. doi: 10.3109/07434618.2012.716079 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Binger C, Kent-Walsh J, Berens J, del Campo S, & Rivera D (2008). Teaching Latino parents to support the multi-symbol message productions of their children who require AAC . Augmentative and Alternative Communication , 24 , 323–338. doi: 10.1080/07434610802130978 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Binger C, & Light J (2007). The effect of aided AAC modeling on the expression of multi-symbol messages by preschoolers who use AAC . Augmentative and Alternative Communication , 23 , 30–43. doi: 10.1080/07434610600807470 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Binger C, Maguire-Marshall M, & Kent-Walsh J (2011). Using aided AAC models, recasts, and contrastive targets to teach grammatical morphemes to children who use AAC . Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research , 54 , 160–176. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0163) [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Boenisch J & Soto G (2015). The oral core vocabulary of typically developing English-speaking school-aged children: Implications for AAC practice . Augmentative and Alternative Communication , 31 , 77–84. doi: 10.3109/07434618.2014.1001521 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Clendon S, Erickson K, van Rensburg RJ, & Amm J (2014). Shared storybook reading – An authentic context for developing literacy, language, and communication skills . SIG 12 Perspectives on Augmentative and Alternative Communication , 23 ( 4 ), 182–191. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dahlgren Sandberg A (2002) Phonological recoding problems in children with severe congenital speech impairments — The importance of productive speech. In Witruk E, Friederici AD, & Lachmann T (Eds). Basic functions of language, reading and reading disability. Neuropsychology and Cognition , 20 . Boston, MA: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-1011-6_19 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dickinson DK, McCabe A, Anastasopoulos L, Peisner-Feinberg ES, & Poe MD (2003). The comprehensive language approach to early literacy: The interrelationships among vocabulary, phonological sensitivity, and print knowledge among preschool-aged children . Journal of Educational Psychology , 95 ( 3 ), 465–481. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.3.465 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ehri LC (2000). Learning to read and learning to spell . Topics in Language Disorders , 20 ( 3 ), 19–36. doi: 10.1097/00011363-200020030-00005 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fallon KA, Light J, McNaughton D, Drager K, & Hammer C (2004). The effects of direct instruction on the single-word reading skills of children who require augmentative and alternative communication . Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research , 47 , 1424–1439. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/106) [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fenson L, Dale PS, Reznick JS, Bates E, Thal DJ, Pethick SJ, Tomasello M, Mervis CB, & Stiles J (1994). Variability in early communicative development . Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development , 59 , 1–185. https://www.jstor.org/stable/1166093 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Flyvbjerg B (2006). Five misunderstandings about case-study research . Qualitative Inquiry , 12 , 219–245. doi: 10.1177/1077800405284363 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fuchs D & Fuchs L (2006). Introduction to Response to Intervention: What, why, and how valid is it? Reading Research Quarterly , 41 , 93–99. doi: https://www.jstor.org/stable/4151803 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Geist L, & Erickson K (2016). Core vocabulary AAC instruction for students with significant cognitive disabilities . Presentation at the Annual Conference of the Assistive Technology Industry Association (ATIA), Orlando, FL . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gerber S & Kraat A (1992). Use of a developmental model of language acquisition: Applications to children using AAC systems . Augmentative and Alternative Communication , 8 , 19–32. doi: 10.1080/07434619212331276013 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Granlund M, Björck- Åkesson E, Wilder J, & Ylvén R (2008). AAC interventions for children in a family environment: Implementing evidence in practice . Augmentative and Alternative Communication , 24 , 207–219. doi: 10.1080/08990220802387935 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hamburg M, & Collins F (2010). The path to personalized medicine . New England Journal of Medicine , 363 , 301–304. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp1006304 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hemphill L & Tivnan T (2008). The importance of early vocabulary for literacy achievement in high-poverty schools . Journal for Education of Students Placed at Risk , 13 , 426–451. doi: 10.1080/10824660802427710 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kasari C, Kaiser A, Goods K, Nietfeld J, Mathy P, Landa R, Murphy S, & Almirall D (2014). Communication interventions for minimally verbal children with autism: A sequential multiple assignment randomized trial . Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry , 53 , 635–646. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.01.019 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kent-Walsh J, Murza K, Malani M & Binger C (2015). Effects of communication partner instruction on the communication of individuals using AAC: A meta-analysis . Augmentative and Alternative Communication , 31 , 271–284. doi: 10.3109/07434618.2015.1052153 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Laubscher E & Light J (2020). Core vocabulary lists for young children and considerations for early language development: A narrative review . Augmentative and Alternative Communication , 36 , 43–53. doi: 10.1080/07434618.2020.1737964 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Light J, Collier B, & Parnes P (1985). Communicative interaction between young nonspeaking physically disabled children and their primary caregivers: Part III-Modes of communication . Augmentative and Alternative Communication , 1 , 125–133. doi: 10.1080/07434618512331273621 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Light J & McNaughton D (2009). Accessible Literacy Learning: Evidence-based reading instruction for individuals with autism, cerebral palsy, Down syndrome, and other disabilities . San Diego, CA: Mayer Johnson [ Google Scholar ]
  • Light J & McNaughton D (2012). Supporting the communication, language, and literacy development of children with complex communication needs: State of the science and future research priorities . Assistive Technology , 24 , 34–44. doi: 10.1080/10400435.2011.648717 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Light J & McNaughton D (2014). Communicative competence for individuals who require augmentative and alternative communication: A new definition for a new era of communication . Augmentative and Alternative Communication , 30 , 1–18. doi: 10.3109/07434618.2014.885080 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Light J & McNaughton D (2017). Improving literacy outcomes for individuals with autism spectrum disorder and limited speech . Webcast retrieved from https://aac-learning-center.psu.edu/2017/09/30/improving-literacy-outcomes-for-individuals-with-asd/
  • Light J, & McNaughton D (2020) Literacy intervention for individuals with complex communication needs. In Beukelman DR & Light JC (2020). Augmentative and alternative communication: Supporting children and adults with complex communication needs . (pp. 427–481). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Machalicek W, Sanford A, Lang R, Rispoli M, Molfenter N, & Mbesha M (2010). Literacy interventions for students with physical and developmental disabilities who use aided AAC devices: A systematic review . Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities , 22 , 219–240. doi: 10.1007/s10882-009-9175-3 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mandak K, Light J, & Boyle S (2018). The effects of instruction on single-word reading for individuals who use aided AAC: A systematic review . Augmentative and Alternative Communication , 34 , 206–218. doi: 10.1080/07434618.2018.1470668 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mandak K, O’Neill T, Light J, & Fosco G (2017). Bridging the gap from values to actions: A family systems framework for family-centered AAC services . Augmentative and Alternative Communication , 33 , 32–41. doi: 10.1080/07434618.2016.1271453 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Marchman V & Bates E (1994). Continuity in lexical and morphological development: A test of the critical mass hypothesis . Journal of Child Language , 21 , 339–366. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900009302 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McNaughton D & Beukelman DR (2010). Transition strategies for adolescents and young adults who use AAC . Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Millar D, Light J, & McNaughton D (2004). The effect of direct instruction and Writer’s Workshop on the early writing skills of children who use Augmentative and Alternative Communication . Augmentative and Alternative Communication , 20 , 164–178. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Millar D, Light J, & Schlosser R (2006). The impact of augmentative and alternative communication intervention on the speech production of individuals with developmental disabilities: A research review . Journal of Speech Language Hearing Research , 49 , 248–264. doi: 10.1080/07434610410001699690 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Molarte A, Rosenbaum A, & von Noorden G (1991). Clinical characteristics and surgical treatment of intermittent esotropia . Journal of Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus , 28 , 137–142. doi: 10.3928/0191-3913-19910501-06 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Newborg J (2005). Battelle Developmental Inventory, Second Edition . Itasca, IL: Riverside. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nonnekes J & Nieuwboer A (2018). Towards personalized rehabilitation for gait impairments in Parkinson’s disease . Journal of Parkinson’s Disease , 8 , s101–s106. doi: 10.3233/JPD-181464 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Oommen E & McCarthy J (2015). Simultaneous natural speech and AAC interventions for children with childhood apraxia of speech: Lessons from a speech language pathologist focus group . Augmentative and Alternative Commnication , 31 , 63–76. doi: 10.3109/07434618.2014.1001520 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • O’Neill T, Light J, & Pope L (2018). Effects of interventions that include aided AAC input on the communication of individuals with complex communication needs: A meta-analysis . Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research , 61 , 1743–1765. doi: 10.1044/2018_jslhr-l-17-0132 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Project Core (2020). Universal core communication systems . Center for Literacy and Disability Studies, University of North Carolina, NC . Retrieved from http://www.project-core.com/communication-systems/ [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reichle J, & Drager K (2010). Examining issues of aided communication display and navigational strategies for young children with developmental disabilities . Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities , 22 , 289–311. doi: 10.1007/s10882-010-9191-3 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Romski MA, & Sevcik RA (2005). Augmentative communication and early intervention: Myths and realities . Infants & Young Children , 18 , 174–185. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Romski MA, Sevcik RA, Adamson LB, Cheslock M, Smith A, Barker RM, & Bakeman R (2010). Randomized comparison of augmented and nonaugmented language interventions for toddlers with developmental delays and their parents . Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research , 53 , 350–364. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0156) [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Romski MA, Sevcik R, Barton-Hulsey A, & Whitmore A (2015). Early intervention and AAC: What a difference 30 years makes . Augmentative and Alternative Communication , 31 , 181–202. doi: 10.3109/07434618.2015.1064163 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sigafoos J, O’Reilly M, Ganz J, Lancioni G, & Schlosser R (2005). Supporting self-determination in AAC interventions by assessing preferences for communication devices . Technology and Disability , 17 , 143–153. doi: 10.3233/TAD-2005-17302 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stanovich K (2008). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy . The Journal of Education , 189 ( 1/2 ), 23–55. doi: http://www.jstor.org/stable/42748659 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tibbling L (1969). The rotatory nystagmus response in children . Acta Oto-Laryngologica , 68 , 1–6. doi: 10.3109/00016486909121586 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • van der Meer L, Sigafoos J, O’Reilly M, & Lancioni G (2011). Assessing preferences for AAC options in communication interventions for individuals with developmental disabilities: A review of the literature . Research in Developmental Disabilities , 32 , 1422–1431. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.02.003 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Warren SF, & Brady NC (2007). The role of maternal responsivity in the development of children with intellectual disabilities . Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews , 13 , 330–338. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20177 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wilkinson K, & Albert A (2001). Adaptations of fast mapping for vocabulary intervention with augmented language users . Augmentative and Alternative Communication , 17 , 120–132. doi: 10.1080/aac.17.2.120.132 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wilkinson K, & McIlvane W (2002). Considerations in teaching graphic symbols to beginning communicators. In Reichle J, Beukelman DR, & Light J (Eds.), Exemplary practices for beginning communicators: Implications for AAC (pp. 273–321). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zimmerman IL, Steiner VG, & Pond E (2011). Preschool Language Scale 5th Edition (PLS-5) . San Antonio, TX: Pearson [ Google Scholar ]

Communication Community

Best Children's Books for Speech and Language Development

Maybe you are a parent wondering how to choose from the thousands and thousands of books on the market. Maybe you are a therapist wondering what books you should use to promote language development with your students. Well... let’s talk about literacy!

Read below to find out how you can use different styles of books to promote language development. From pop-up, to tactile, to sound books, and more - we've got them covered (links too)!

I make it a point that when I speak with parents, I tell them that books and joint book reading can be one of the most beneficial ways they can spend time with their children. Books are something I recommend to parents more than most other at-home resources. Joint book reading can also be a very intimate experience between you and your child. For example, it often follows a predictable routine, such as getting ready for bed. Reading with or to your child promotes interactive behaviors that foster a strong sense of communication and social closeness. Interactive behaviors include turn-taking, imitation, establishing joint attention, and developing anticipatory sets ( Paul & Norbury, 2012 ) . Even if your child is not yet a "reader," exposure to books and other print also encourages early literacy awareness.

books letters and stories communicate without speech

You may think, my child is less than 12 months, why should I expose them to text and language they cannot yet understand ? Good question. In literacy development, there are numerous precursors that serve as tools to promote reading and writing skills down the road. One precursor is literacy socialization . In other words - exposure. By reading to your one-year-old, you are modeling the appropriate way to hold a book, turn the pages, and have an opportunity to point to and label various nouns (characters, places, objects). As your child gets older, they will begin to understand that book reading contains a particular sequence and they will start to make predictions as they become more familiar with literary structures and story elements (characters, setting, main idea, etc.). It is an ever-evolving process as your child grows and expands-on these skills, but beginning young is one of the best indicators for future literacy success.

As you expose your children to print and other literary elements, there are a plethora of books on the market that can serve as incredibly helpful resources. I have included some below that can be used to teach and model specific concepts in therapy, or to use at home. These books include pop-up/lift-the-flap books, tactile (touch and feel) books, animated (sound) books, wordless picture books, rhyming books, and alphabet books. Check them out!

Pop-up / Lift-the-Flap Books

books letters and stories communicate without speech

Pop-up or moveable books provide children with the opportunity to engage with fun, tangible features revolving around their favorite story. Pop-up books incorporate things like pull tabs and flaps that are found throughout the book and are usually easier to manipulate; they are most commonly used by pre- or early readers. Books such as these may also sustain your child’s attention longer since they contain a variety of hands-on components. In therapy, I use pop-up books when teaching positional concepts and prepositions. This gives your child frequent exposure to words like in, on, under, next to, etc. You can also incorporate prepositions into a variety of directions - promoting receptive understanding and motor planning (e.g., “Show me the ball behind the tree.”). Additionally, pop-up books can also teach cause and effect concepts (e.g., pulling down the bottom flap will make the dog go under the covers; pushing up a tab will make the dog jump).

Pop-up / lift-the-flap books to check out:

- Where’s Spot? by Eric Hill

- Dear Zoo by Rod Campbell

- Zoom Zoom Baby by Karen Katz

Tactile Books

Tactile books are also sometimes called “Touch and Feel” books. They are designed to be very hands-on and explore different types of sensory mediums. Tactile books are great if you are trying to integrate sensory components to your reading routine. They are also a wonderful tool if your child has visual limitations. Again, similar to pop-up books, your child’s time attending to the book is likely to increase because it requires more active engagement when reading the story. It can also provide your child with the additional context needed to engage independently. Think about it: do you think your two-year-old child would be more likely to use their downtime to seek out a book that is visually and tactility stimulating, or a book that has fewer pictures and more text? I know that when I was a kid, the fun was in ripping the “boring” text-heavy books, not “reading" them. Further, tactile books provide very concrete examples of a variety of describing words (e.g., shiny, rough, fluffy ), which can increase your child’s vocabulary acquisition. As your child increases their descriptive repertoire, it can promote additional language to help increase the structure and length of their phrases and sentences.

Tactile books to check out:

- Bright Baby Touch and Feel ( Words, Colors, Numbers, Shapes ) by Roger Pridy

- Feely Bugs by David A. Carter

- Pat the Bunny by Dorothy Kunhardt

Sound (animated) Books

Sensory, sensory, sensory! See a similar pattern for some of these children’s books? Another sensory component that children tend to gravitate towards when it comes to books is sound. Books with sound promote auditory engagement and show children that reading can be fun (and sound fun too)! Many of these books also contain other versions without the audio feature, which can be used as your child begins to increase their literacy skills and read independently. Sound books are also one of my favorite tools for younger children who are just beginning to combine words (e.g., “cow moo”; “doggie wuff”). Many sound books contain animal themes or features, which kids are usually eager to talk about and describe. Some animal sounds are not as articulatory intensive (hard to say) as other common words; so words like moo moo and baa baa are easier to combine and use with frequency.

Sound books to check out:

- Polar Bear, Polar Bear, What Do You Hear? By Bill Martin Jr. and Eric Carle

- We’re Going on a Bear Hunt by Michael Rosen

- Simple First Words: Let’s Talk by Roger Priddy

Wordless Picture Books

Wordless picture books are a must-have in speech-language therapy. Though it does not promote the same level of print/alphabet exposure as most early-literacy books, these books facilitate more room for language expansion and imagination. I use these books to target an assortment of skills, such as labeling, predicting, and emotional inferencing (just to name a few). Because wordless picture books contain little to no text, it also provides you with more opportunities to directly interact with your child and create language-learning opportunities. Asking questions like, “How do you think [character] feels?” or “Where do you think the treasure is hidden?” can prompt your child to form social inferences and test our their theories. You can also use books like these to indirectly measure your child’s language expansion. How are they describing some of the illustrations that they see at three years, as compared to how they are describing at four?  

Wordless picture books to check out:

- Good Dog Carl by Alexandra Day

- A Boy, a Dog, and a Frog by Mercer Mayer

- The Red Book by Barbara Lehman

Rhyming Books

books letters and stories communicate without speech

Some of the most popular authors who writing children's rhyming books include Bill Martin Jr., Alan Katz, Jan Thomas, Anna Dewdney, and Dr. Seuss. Some of these great books include Rhyming Dust Bunnies and Llama Llama Red Pajama .  There are rhyming books for a variety of different reading levels, so you can find different ones as your loved one’s literacy skills develop. Like those literacy precursors ( phonological awareness ) that were mentioned before, rhyming is foundational to reading and writing skills. It teaches children that letters within words function as very specific units of speech and changes in sound can alter an entire meaning of a word. It also teaches kids the concepts of same and different, as it relates to discriminating words and sounds. As your child develops their rhyming skills, books such as these can also increase in interactiveness - have your child guess what word will come next based on the rhyme, or have them create their own.

Rhyming books to check out:

- Llama Llama Red Pajama by Anna Dewdeny

- How do Dinosaurs Say Goodnight? By Jane Yolen & Mark Teague

- Goodnight Moon by Margaret Wise Brown

... and lastly, Alphabet Books

books letters and stories communicate without speech

Alphabet books are (most of time) designed to teach your child how to learn the letters in the alphabet in a fun, illustrative way. They are geared towards young children because alphabet books are styled in a way that promotes letter-name and letter-sound knowledge; specifically, phoneme awareness. For example, it is not uncommon to hear a child who is not yet reading say something like, “ A is for a pple!” This child may not have a complete understanding of letter-name correspondence; however, they are developing an awareness of print and making learned associations. Children are also increasing their own letter-sound correspondence when they can label apple , and see a big ‘A’ as its counterpart.

Alphabet books to check out:

- ABC: An Amazing Alphabet Book by Dr. Seuss

- Chicka Chicka Boom Boom by Bill Martin Jr. & John Archambault

- Curious George Learns the Alphabet by H.A. Rey

These are just some of the many, many books that can be used when reading to your child. If your child shows a particular interest in a specific book or genre (e.g., superheroes), you can still apply these ideas to them as well!

books letters and stories communicate without speech

Looking for more resources? We have you covered!

Anti-Racism resources (including books)

Best toys for speech and language development

Board games for speech and language

This post contains some affiliate links and we may be (slightly) compensated if you use them, but all opinions are our own. We appreciate the support!

You might also like

AAC Core Word of the Week Resource: Lesson Slides AND Companion Packet

AAC Core Word of the Week Resource: Lesson Slides AND Companion Packet

How to Write Stuttering Goals [with goal bank]

How to Write Stuttering Goals [with goal bank]

10 Easy Spring-Themed Activities for Speech Therapy

10 Easy Spring-Themed Activities for Speech Therapy

Spring Short Stories with WH Comprehension Questions

Spring Short Stories with WH Comprehension Questions

Community Office Hours

Community Office Hours

Subscribe to new posts., subscribe to be notified of new content and support communication community, help keep this site independent..

Communication Community

Book cover

Narrative Persuasion. A Cognitive Perspective on Language Evolution pp 111–138 Cite as

Stories Without Language

  • Francesco Ferretti 41  
  • First Online: 23 August 2022

228 Accesses

Part of the book series: Interdisciplinary Evolution Research ((IDER,volume 7))

The chapter addresses the issue of the expressive systems by which actual communication takes shape, with a twofold aim: showing that it is possible to tell stories without language and claiming that narrative communication is the evolutionary precondition of language emergence. This aim entails understanding what kind of expressive system was better suited for conveying the mental stories inhabiting human thoughts. The hypothesis is that pantomime was such a system since it embodied the narrative features capable of setting the stage for a narrative form of communication. If pantomime allowed archaic hominins to persuade by telling stories without language, then modern humans employed complex grammatical systems to enhance their persuasive aims through argumentation. The transition between these two forms of communication is framed within a continuity perspective, in which modern language is a reconstruction of old communicative forms.

  • Conversation
  • Argumentation
  • Gesture-first hypothesis
  • Grammaticalization

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution .

Buying options

  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
  • Durable hardcover edition

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Ahlner F, Zlatev J (2010) Cross-modal iconicity: a cognitive semiotic approach to sound symbolism. Sign Syst Stud 38(1/4):298–348

Article   Google Scholar  

Arbib M (2012) How the brain got language: towards a new road map. Oxford University Press, Oxford

Book   Google Scholar  

Asher N, Lascarides A (2013) Strategic conversation. Semant Pragmat 6(2):1–62

Google Scholar  

Barnard A (2016) Language in prehistory. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

Benítez-Burraco A, Ferretti F, Progovac L (2021) Human self-domestication and the evolution of pragmatics. Cogn Sci 45(6):e12987

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Berwick RC, Chomsky N (2016) Why only us: language and evolution. MIT Press, Cambridge

Bickerton D (1990) Language and species. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

Bickerton D (1995) Language and human behavior. Washington University Press, Seattle

Bickerton D (2009) Adam’s tongue: how humans made language, how language made humans. Macmillan, New York

Botne R (2006) Motion time and tense on the grammaticalization of come and go to future markers in Bantu. Stud Afr Linguist 35(2):127–188

Boyd B (2009) On the origin of stories: evolution, cognition, and fiction. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

Boyd B (2018) The evolution of stories: from mimesis to language, from fact to fiction. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci 9(1):e1444

Burge T (1993) Content preservation. Philos Rev 102(4):457–488

Cassell J, McNeill D (1991) Gesture and the poetics of prose. Poetics Today 12(3):375–404

Chomsky N (1988) Language and problems of knowledge. The Managua lectures. MIT Press, Cambridge

Chomsky N (2010) Some simple Evo-devo theses: how true might they be for language? In: Larson RK, Déprez V, Yamakido H (eds) The evolution of language. Biolinguistic perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 45–62

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Corballis MC (2002) From hand to mouth: the origins of language. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

Corballis MC (2011) The recursive mind: the origins of human language, thought, and civilization. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

Corballis MC (2015) The wandering mind: what the brain does when you’re not looking. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

Corballis MC (2017) The truth about language: what it is and where it came from. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

Davidson D (1984) Radical interpretation. In: Davidson D (ed) Inquiries into truth and interpretation. Clarendon Press, Oxford, pp 125–140

De Waal F (2016) Are we smart enough to know how smart animals are? WW Norton & Company, New York

Dingemanse M, Blasi DE, Lupyan G, Christiansen MH, Monaghan P (2015) Arbitrariness, iconicity and systematicity in language. Trends Cogn Sci 19(10):603–613

Donald M (1991) Origins of the modern mind: three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

Donald M (2001) A mind so rare: the evolution of human consciousness. WW Norton & Company, New York

Eldredge N, Gould SJ (1972) Punctuated equilibria: an alternative to phyletic gradualism. In: Schopf TM, Freeman TJM (eds) Models in paleobiology. Cooper & Co, San Francisco, pp 82–115

Ferretti F (2015) La facoltà di linguaggio. Determinanti biologiche e variabilità culturale. Carocci, Roma

Ferretti F (2016) The social brain is not enough: on the importance of the ecological brain for the origin of language. Front Psychol 7:1138

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Ferretti F (2019) Quali precursori per il linguaggio? La comunicazione umana tra adattamento, exaptation ed evoluzione culturale. Sistemi Intelligenti 31(1):139–156

Ferretti F (2021) The narrative origins of language. In: Gontier N, Lock A, Sinha C (eds) The Oxford handbook of human symbolic evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford

Ferretti F, Adornetti I (2012) Dalla comunicazione al linguaggio. Scimmie, ominidi e umani in una prospettiva darwiniana. Mondadori Università, Milano

Ferretti F, Adornetti I (2021) Persuasive conversation as a new form of communication in Homo sapiens . Philos Trans R Soc B 376(1824):1–9

Ferretti F, Adornetti I, Chiera A (2022) Narrative pantomime: a protolanguage for persuasive communication. Lingua 271:103247

Fitch WT (2009) Fossil cues to the evolution of speech. In: Botha R, Knight C (eds) The cradle of language. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 112–134

Fitch WT (2017) Empirical approaches to the study of language evolution. Psychon Bull Rev 24(1):3–33

Fodor J (1975) The language of thought. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

Gilbert DT, Malone PS (1995) The correspondence bias. Psychol Bull 117(1):21–38

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Gilbert DT, Krull DS, Malone PS (1990) Unbelieving the unbelievable: some problems in the rejection of false information. J Pers Soc Psychol 59(4):601–613

Grice HP (1989) Studies in the way of words. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

Hamano S (1998) The sound symbolic system of Japanese. CSLI Publications, Stanford

Hauser MD, Yang C, Berwick RC, Tattersall I, Ryan MJ, Watumull J, Chomsky N, Lewontin RC (2014) The mystery of language evolution. Front Psychol 5:401

Heine B, Kuteva T (2002) World lexicon of grammaticalization. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

Heine B, Kuteva T (2007) The genesis of grammar: a reconstruction. Oxford University Press, New York

Hewes GW, Andrew RJ, Carini L, Choe H, Gardner RA, Kortlandt A et al (1973) Primate communication and the gestural origin of language [and comments and reply]. Curr Anthropol 14(1/2):5–24

Hinton L, Nichols J, Ohala JJ (eds) (1994) Sound symbolism. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

Hurford J (2011) The origins of grammar: language in the light of evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford

Jackendoff RS (1992) Languages of the mind. MIT Press, Cambridge

Jackendoff RS (1999) Possible stages in the evolution of the language capacity. Trends Cogn Sci 3(7):272–279

Kendon A (2017) Reflections on the “gesture-first” hypothesis of language origins. Psychon Bull Rev 24(1):163–170

Kirby S, Smith K, Brighton H (2004) From UG to universals: linguistic adaptation through iterated learning studies in language. Found Lang 28(3):587–607

Lascarides A, Asher N (2008) Segmented discourse representation theory: dynamic semantics with discourse structure. In: Bunt H, Muskens R (eds) Computing meaning. Springer, Dordrecht, pp 87–124

Lewis DK (1969) Conventions. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

Lindström J (2009) Interactional linguistics. In: D’hondt S, Östman JO, Verschueren J (eds) The pragmatics of interaction. John Benjamins Publishing Company, Amsterdam, pp 96–103

MacNeilage PF (2008) The origin of speech. Oxford University Press, Oxford

McBride G (2014) Storytelling, behavior planning, and language evolution in context. Front Psychol 5:1131

McNeill D (2005) Introduction. In: McNeill D (ed) Language and gesture. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 1–12

McNeill D (2012) How language began: gesture and speech in human evolution. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

Nuckolls JB (1999) The case for sound symbolism. Annu Rev Anthropol 28(1):225–252

Pedersen J, Fields W (2009) Aspects of repetition in bonobo-human conversation: creating cohesion in a conversation between species. Integr Psychol Behav Sci 43(1):22–41

Perkins M (2007) Pragmatic impairment. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

Perlman M, Gibbs RW (2013) Pantomimic gestures reveal the sensorimotor imagery of a human-fostered gorilla. J Ment Imag 37(3/4):73–96

Perlman M, Clark N, Tanner JE (2014) Iconicity and ape gesture. In: Cartmill EA, Roberts SG, Lyn H, Cornish H (eds) Proceedings of the 10th international conference (EVOLANG10). World Scientific, New Jersey, pp 228–235

Perlman M, Dale R, Lupyan G (2015) Iconicity can ground the creation of vocal symbols. R Soc Open Sci 2(8):150152

Perniss P, Vigliocco G (2014) The bridge of iconicity: from a world of experience to the experience of language. Philos Trans R Soc B 369(1651):20130300

Pickering MJ, Garrod S (2004) Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behav Brain Sci 27(2):169–190

Pinker S (1994) The language instinct. Morrow, New York

Pinker S, Bloom P (1990) Natural language and natural selection. Behav Brain Sci 13(4):707–784

Pinker S, Nowak MA, Lee JJ (2008) The logic of indirect speech. Proc Natl Acad Sci 105(3):833–838

Article   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Poggi I (2008) Iconicity in different types of gestures. Gesture 8(1):45–61

Progovac L (2015) Evolutionary syntax. Oxford University Press, Oxford

Reboul A (2017) Cognition and communication in the evolution of language. Oxford University Press, Cambridge

Russon A, Andrews K (2011a) Orangutan pantomime: elaborating the message. Biol Lett 7:627–630

Russon A, Andrews K (2011b) Pantomime in great apes: evidence and implications. Commun Integr Biol 4(3):315–317

Sacks H, Schegloff EA, Jefferson G (1974) A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language 50(4):696–735

Schegloff EA (1997) Whose text? Whose context? Discourse Soc 8(2):165–187

Shaw-Williams K (2017) The social trackways theory of the evolution of language. Biol Theory 12(4):195–210

Sidnell J (2011) Conversation analysis: an introduction. John Wiley & Sons, Oxford

Sinha C, Gärdenfors P (2014) Time, space, and events in language and cognition: a comparative view. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1326(1):72–81

Sperber D, Wilson D (1986/1995) Relevance. Communication and cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

Sperber D, Clément F, Heintz C, Mascaro O, Mercier H, Origgi G, Wilson D (2010) Epistemic vigilance. Mind Lang 25(4):359–393

Stokoe WC (2001) Language in hand: why sign came before speech. Gallaudet University Press, Washington, DC

Tattersall I (2008) The world from beginnings to 4000 BCE. Oxford University Press, New York

Tattersall I (2010) Human evolution and cognition. Theory Biosci 129(2/3):193–201

Thompson SA, Couper-Kuhlen E (2005) The clause as a locus of grammar and interaction. Discourse Stud 7(4/5):481–505

Thompson SA, Fox BA, Couper-Kuhlen E (2015) Grammar in everyday talk: building responsive actions. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

Tomasello M (2008) Origins of human communication. MIT Press, Cambridge

Tomasello M (2009) Why we cooperate. MIT Press, Cambridge

Tomassello M, Call J (2007) Ape gestures and the origins of language. In: Call J, Tomasello M (eds) The gestural communication of apes and monkeys. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, pp 221–239

Voeltz FKE, Kilian-Hatz C (eds) (2001) Ideophones. Benjamins, Amsterdam

Wacewicz S, Żywiczyński P (2018) Language origins: fitness consequences, platform of trust, cooperation, and turn-taking. Interact Stud 19(1/2):167–182

Wray A (1998) Protolanguage as a holistic system for social interaction. Lang Commun 18(1):47–67

Wray A (2000) Holistic utterances in protolanguage: the link from primates to humans. In: Knight C, Studdert-Kennedy M, Hurford J (eds) The evolutionary emergence of language. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 285–302

Zlatev J (2008) From proto-mimesis to language: evidence from primatology and social neuroscience. J Physiol Paris 102(1/3):137–151

Zlatev J (2014a) Human uniqueness, bodily mimesis and the evolution of language. HUMANA.MENTE J Philos Stud 7(27):197–219

Zlatev J (2014b) Bodily mimesis and the transition to speech. In: Pina M, Gontier N (eds) The evolution of social communication in primates. Interdisciplinary evolution research. Springer, Cham, pp 165–178

Zuberbühler K (2013) Acquired mirroring and intentional communication in primates. Lang Cogn 5(2/3):133–143

Żywiczyński P, Wacewicz S, Sibierska M (2018) Defining pantomime for language evolution research. Topoi 37(2):307–318

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Philosophy, Communication and Performing Arts, Roma Tre University, Rome, Italy

Francesco Ferretti

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2022 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter.

Ferretti, F. (2022). Stories Without Language. In: Narrative Persuasion. A Cognitive Perspective on Language Evolution. Interdisciplinary Evolution Research, vol 7. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-09206-0_5

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-09206-0_5

Published : 23 August 2022

Publisher Name : Springer, Cham

Print ISBN : 978-3-031-09205-3

Online ISBN : 978-3-031-09206-0

eBook Packages : Biomedical and Life Sciences Biomedical and Life Sciences (R0)

Politics & Society

The best books on free speech, recommended by timothy garton ash.

Free Speech: Ten Principles for a Connected World by Timothy Garton Ash

Free Speech: Ten Principles for a Connected World by Timothy Garton Ash

Free speech is the bedrock of a healthy society, but how do we deal with the torrents of horrible comments—and worse—we see on the internet every day? Timothy Garton Ash , author of Free Speech: Ten Principles for A Connected World , outlines a plan for navigating the complexities and recommends the best books to help us think about free speech.

Interview by Sophie Roell , Editor

Free Speech: Ten Principles for a Connected World by Timothy Garton Ash

On Liberty by John Stuart Mill

The best books on Free Speech - How to Cure A Fanatic by Amos Oz

How to Cure A Fanatic by Amos Oz

The best books on Free Speech - Defending My Enemy: American Nazis, the Skokie Case, and the Risks of Freedom by Aryeh Neier

Defending My Enemy: American Nazis, the Skokie Case, and the Risks of Freedom by Aryeh Neier

The best books on Free Speech - Giving Offense: Essays on Censorship by J M Coetzee

Giving Offense: Essays on Censorship by J M Coetzee

The best books on Free Speech - The Master Switch by Tim Wu

The Master Switch by Tim Wu

The best books on Free Speech - On Liberty by John Stuart Mill

1 On Liberty by John Stuart Mill

2 how to cure a fanatic by amos oz, 3 defending my enemy: american nazis, the skokie case, and the risks of freedom by aryeh neier, 4 giving offense: essays on censorship by j m coetzee, 5 the master switch by tim wu.

I n your book, you’ve narrowed it down to ten principles, but free speech is incredibly complicated, isn’t it?

There are multiple states, there are international organisations, there are what I call the ‘private superpowers’—Google, Facebook, Twitter, which, as we’re all discovering, are effectively regulating our freedom of speech, often globally—and then there are other players.

On the other hand, you can still have quite simple principles—liberal principles for free speech in a world where everybody’s becoming neighbours with everybody else. Earlier this year, I presented my ten principles to IFEX, which is the biggest global network of free speech organizations. There was a tremendous amount of enthusiasm because I think a lot of people feel that’s what we need in this incredibly complicated, connected world: a few relatively simple rules of the road.

I tend to think of free speech as one of various freedoms that we enjoy in a democracy, but you make the point that it’s of more central importance than that: free speech is the freedom on which all the other freedoms are based.

It is the oxygen of all other freedoms. The classic example of this is Amartya Sen’s famous study , which shows that there’s never been a major famine in a country which had a free press—because the news gets out and there’s outrage. So there’s this elemental connection even with the right to life, to have enough to eat.

Readers can have a look at all ten of your principles here , but, briefly, what is free speech?

If you look at the ten, you’ll see that beyond the basic principle that we need freedom of speech, the next most important thing I formulate as, “We neither make threats of violence nor accept violent intimidation.” We all know this because of the murder of the Charlie Hebdo cartoonist, the fatwas and the death threats. All over the world one of the most important threats to free speech are threats of violence. It’s as important to face down what I call ‘the assassin’s veto’—as in the case of Charlie Hebdo —as it is not yourself to make threats of violence. They’re two sides of the same coin.

Fundamentally, if we could agree on those two—the basic principle that we need effective freedom of speech and the principle of no violence—then we could argue about what are the legitimate limits in relation to privacy or religion or national security, or how we talk about diversity, hate speech, and so on. All the others are areas where we can have an argument, but at least we’ve defined the big areas for debate.

I hadn’t appreciated is to the extent there are big differences between countries, even in the West. On paper, America has by far the freest speech, more so than various European countries, but they also have quite a bit of variation between them.

People in China , or in India, if you talk to them, imagine there is ‘the West’ but actually there are big differences inside the West. In the US, there is the First Amendment tradition, which is undoubtedly the greatest constitutionally anchored tradition of free speech in the world. But the United States has no hate speech laws. Most European countries and Canada have hate speech laws. That’s one big transatlantic difference.

“Through Facebook and Google and Twitter, American norms are being spread worldwide”

Let’s talk a bit more about some of these themes in the context of the books you’ve chosen. The first on your list is John Stuart Mill ’s On Liberty  (1859). Was he the first to argue for free speech?

Absolutely not. You can find notions of free speech not just in Ancient Greece—where a massive amount of what we think of as free speech and democracy comes from—but, interestingly, in ancient Chinese texts, in ancient Indian texts, in the edicts of the Emperor Ashoka. It’s really important to say that the idea has been around forever, and not just in western culture.

But in the modern western world, you start in the 17th century with the English Revolution, with John Milton, then with the Enlightenment—English, French, Scottish, and American. Then you go, on the one hand, to the First Amendment in the U.S., which is obviously a classic statement of free speech, and in England, to John Stuart Mill.

In my view, Mill is one of those mildly irritating authors like Tocqueville, who say so much so well that it’s difficult to say it better. Actually, when I say On Liberty , I mean above all chapter two of On Liberty , “Of the Liberty of Thought and Discussion” which says so much, so brilliantly, so eloquently…

The free speech that we enjoy today, how does it compare to Mill’s ideas? I mean, is he advocating for freer speech than we actually have now, or are we pretty much in line with what he envisaged?

We where? We in the West, we in Britain? Which country are we talking about?

Most of our readers are in the US or the UK.

That’s a rather good question. The answer is interestingly complicated, because, of course, a question of free speech is also a question about power relations, who is in a position to speak freely? The ideal of free speech is an ideal of equality, where everyone is free to speak freely. Now of course, the world of Victorian Britain didn’t have that. Did the servants have freedom of speech? Did women have freedom of speech? Did colonised people in the British Empire have effective freedom of speech? Certainly not. So in that sense, we have more, because more people have more right to speak freely—and more ability, because so many of us have a smartphone or a computer.

“The mark of a free society is that we restrain ourselves”

On the other hand, I think Mill would be extremely worried by some of the taboos we see today, the sense that we have to tiptoe around all sorts of really difficult subjects. I don’t think he would have been keen at all on hate speech laws, because one of the key things he said—and where I am very much a Millian—is that the mark of a free society is that we restrain ourselves. The state is not the father telling you, like a child, what you can do and what you can’t do, and putting you in the corner. Mature, adult citizens make their own choices, and we choose what I call ‘robust civility.’ That’s exactly what he thinks, but he insists very strongly that it shouldn’t be imposed by law.

In terms of his argument, he says we should be free to say anything, “however immoral it might be considered.”

Yes, and that the criteria should not be mere offence—he’s very good on that. But his central statement is about seeking the truth. What he says is very original, which is that many false statements may contain a grain of truth, and even an utterly false statement challenges us to restate our position. It’s therefore a way to keep the good sword of truth sharp, if you’re constantly confronting it with other arguments. He talks about the ‘deep slumber’ of a decided opinion, of received wisdom. That’s at the heart of what he’s trying to argue—the argument from seeking the truth.

He also has this wonderful passage where he says that we’re so much shaped by the world we’re in that the same causes which make someone a churchman in London would have made him a Buddhist or Confucian in Peking. That’s so profoundly true. We’re so shaped by the environment, that you need that contrary, idiosyncratic opinion to shake it up.

And yet he doesn’t think you should be allowed to harm others with your speech. How is that enforced, then?

This is Mill’s ‘harm principle’—that I should be free to say or do anything, so long as it does not do harm to others. That’s core to modern liberalism, the basic framework. Then the argument becomes, ‘What harms other people?’

So take the torrent of horrible stuff—rubbish, abuse, hate speech—which is flowing through the internet. As I say in the book, the internet is the largest sewer in human history, and the sewerage is all waiting to spill out of your smart phone. The question is, what’s genuinely harmful in that?

Now, that’s very difficult to work out because, as with so much else with free speech, context is all. If I started ranting to you about Tutsis in Rwanda here, sitting at a table in north Oxford, it would be stupid, but it wouldn’t have harmful effects. In the context of Rwanda in 1994, people got killed as a result.

Nonetheless, it’s immensely clarifying to start with the question about harm, and it’s harm , not mere offence. One of the diseases of our time is that people are saying, ‘You shouldn’t say that!’—just because it’s merely offensive to somebody. It’s what I call the offensiveness veto. You almost get to the point where just one person has to stand up and say, ‘I’m offended’ for a speaker to be disinvited from a university.

But how do you stop harm happening? So really horrible stuff is being said about the Tutsis on Radio Télévision Libre des Mille Collines in Rwanda. What do you then do? Does the state get involved?

Yes. This is a simple distinction, but I think it’s a really, really important one to bring into the debate. At the moment, we have this cauldron we call ‘hate speech.’ Within that is really, really dangerous stuff, which ends up with people being killed or silenced, as well as just very offensive stuff, or rubbish, or stupidity. What you have to do is take apart the ingredients of this stew and say which parts we need the state to go after. The state should go after what I call ‘dangerous speech’—something that is intended and likely to lead to physical violence or serious psychological harm.

Hate speech as such—hateful speech—I say we have to counter in civil society, by calling people out on it in everyday life. People say something stupid about Muslims, you call them out on it. People make a stupid racist joke, you call them out on it, either online or in real life.

“What we’re all trying to do is to teach our children to navigate the high seas of the internet”

But no. 1, in principle it shouldn’t be the state having to organise all that, because then we’re children back in nursery school. No. 2, because there is so much more speech as a result of the internet—just oceans of it—the state is totally incapable of policing all of that. So the state should focus on the really dangerous stuff.

For dangerous speech, people endlessly quote Oliver Wendell Holmes: “The most stringent protection of free speech would not protect a man in falsely shouting fire in a theatre and causing a panic.” That’s a pretty silly thing to do, but Mill’s example is much better. He says that it was fine to criticise corn dealers—who were the, I don’t know, investment bankers of his day—but if someone is inciting an angry mob to violence outside the corn merchant’s house, that’s a different matter. It’s a very simple image, but it gets you started on what we mean by dangerous speech as opposed to hate speech.

One sentence jumped out at me from your book: “The devil is in the details.” Before I started reading it, I just didn’t grasp the amount of detail and thought you have to go into, at so many levels, to figure out what a good framework is.

Absolutely. But, at the same time, that’s why you’ve got ten simple principles. The metaphor that runs through my book is a metaphor of navigation. Michel Foucault quotes an ancient Greek philosopher, saying that we should teach free speech like navigation. It’s a wonderful image. What we’re all trying to do is to teach our children to navigate the high seas of the internet. These are very high and often quite rough seas. You’ve got to start with a few basic principles of navigation—so you have your Pole star in the north—and then go into the detail.

I had a really bad experience setting up a Facebook page to help some Syrian refugees. Some of the comments were so awful, I couldn’t bear it. I wasn’t brave, calling people out on it—I just never opened it again.

So next on the list, you’ve got a book by the Israeli novelist Amos Oz, How to Cure a Fanatic  (2004) . Why have you chosen this?

I absolutely love this book, firstly because it’s a beautifully written and very funny short book, and secondly because I think humour is unbelievably important as a way we use free speech to live with diversity. The Today programme, on BBC Radio 4, is full of mild ethnic joshing between the English, the Welsh, and the Scots. That’s the sign of a really healthy society, when you’ve got to the point that you can joke about it.

In my book I quote something terribly interesting, which is that in Senegal, which is a very diverse society, there are actually rituals of inter-ethnic joking. Everybody does jokes, and then, when they’re asked, ‘Why is it that people get on quite well with their neighbours?’ a large proportion of them say, ‘Because of these joking rituals.’ What Amoz Oz says is, ‘I have never met a fanatic who has a sense of humour, or someone with a sense of humour who is a fanatic.’ And therefore, he says, he wants to manufacture humour pills and have them distributed free around the Middle East. I think that’s just such a great insight.

He writes: “Fanaticism is unfortunately an ever-pressing component of human nature, an evil gene if you like.” I thought that was quite interesting, this idea that we all have a tendency toward fanaticism that has to be reined in.

Yes, but what’s so wonderful about the book, is it’s showing how dialogue, debate, free interaction, is one of the best ways of dealing with that. I quote in my book this wonderful song by Nina Simone, “I Wish I Knew How it Would Feel to be Free” with the key line, ‘I wish you could know what it means to be me,’ and that’s it. There are two sides: you know what it means to be me , so it’s both the speaker and the listener. The way we live with difference is first of all by understanding where the other person is coming from.

There’s a Quaker saying which is perhaps a little bit idealistic but nonetheless rather beautiful, which is, ‘An enemy is a friend whose story you haven’t heard.’ Now, obviously that’s not true everywhere in all circumstances. If you’re facing the SS, you have to reach for your gun. Nonetheless, there’s a deep truth in there.

“There’s a Quaker saying: An enemy is a friend whose story you haven’t heard.”

The idea that you can manage a multicultural society, a society with people from everywhere speaking all languages, all faiths, all belief systems, by telling everybody to shut up seems to me profoundly superficial and illusory. The way you do it is by getting people to speak about their differences, even very difficult subjects, but in a context of robust civility. And humour is a great lubricant.

Oz has this wonderful passage where he quotes A Life of Brian , where the crowd chants, “We are all individuals, we are all individuals,” in unison, and then one person says, “I’m not, I’m not,” and they all try and shut him up. But he is one of those people, because he is this solitary voice. He’s one of those people holding out against an incredibly powerful consensus.

What’s he arguing for?

He’s absolutely for a two-state solution, peace now, although I suspect now he thinks it’s probably a hopeless cause.

Are fanatics generally a problem for free speech? Say in the context of Brexit . Some of the Brexiteers are so noisy that it gives this illusion that there’s huge demand for a hard Brexit. Doesn’t free speech favour the people who are talking the loudest?

I think there’s a more subtle point there. Free speech is not just about the laws you have, good or bad. It’s about the whole structure of communication. We’ve got into a situation now in the UK where it privileges the voices that shout. Partly that’s because we have the tabloids we have—the Eurosceptic press have been selling this really deceptive and mendacious narrative but selling it very powerfully. It’s also because of the echo chamber effect from the internet. Then you have the fact that the internet has simply cut the feet from under the business model of most newspapers. Therefore all newspapers are fighting for their life. They’re drowning. What do you do when you’re drowning? You wave and shout.So they’re all waving and shouting, ‘Come over here, give us your clicks!’ (and the advertising revenue that comes with those).

So we have a media landscape which—apart from the BBC, which becomes even more important—privileges the shouting voice. If it bleeds, it leads, if it roars, it scores. Funnily enough, I have had this experience with my own Guardian column. I’ve been writing a Guardian column for 15 years or so. The clicks you get sometimes are just amazing. Guardian online has a monthly audience of 40 million, so a recent one I did had a quarter of a million views. It’s fantastic.

“We’ve got into a situation now where it privileges the voices that shout”

Let’s go on to book three on your list, Defending My Enemy  (1979) by Aryeh Neier.

This is a most incredible story. Aryeh Neier is himself a Holocaust survivor. His family, who were Jewish, got out of Germany pretty much at the last minute. In the late 1970s he was running the ACLU— the American Civil Liberties Union—and decided to defend the right of a bunch of neo-Nazis to march through a town called Skokie, where a very large number of Holocaust survivors lived. You can imagine this was massively controversial. He got hate mail, many people resigned from the ACLU.

This book tells the story of that. What I find particularly moving is the first chapter, when he explains why he does it, and he says—I paraphrase—‘It’s not in spite of being Jewish, it’s precisely because I’m a Jewish Holocaust survivor that I know that free speech and the law is the defence of the weak against the strong. And if I ask that for myself, I have to ask it also for others, and so that’s why I’m defending my enemy.’

“How would I have behaved if I’d been an East German? Would I have been a dissident or a collaborator?”

So it’s not just a very coherent and powerful argument, it’s a very moving argument coming from the guy who’s writing it—and he went on, by the way, to be a terrific international human rights activist. He headed George Soros’s Open Society Foundation for many, many years. He has practised what he preached. But this is such a defining moment, and I remember my friend Christopher Hitchens saying to me that the Skokie case was one of the things that made him want to move to the United States. Those were the days when the United States really stood as a beacon for free speech and civil liberties.

Would you have done the same as Aryeh Neier?

You have to take the question in two parts. Part number one is the kind of question I asked myself when I was writing about my Stasi file which is how would I have behaved if I’d been an East German? Would I have been a dissident or a collaborator? I don’t know the answer to that question, how I would have behaved. Part two is, in principle, do I think he was right? Absolutely I think he was right.

What do you make of people who then cancelled their ACLU membership on the back of this? Do you think that’s reasonable?

A perfectly legitimate choice. You disagree with this profoundly, that’s fine. This is Milton, it’s Mill, it’s Aryeh Neier. In the famous formula, we must defend the thought we hate, not just the thoughts we like. I mean, in contemporary terms, I remember Theresa May, when she was Home Secretary, once saying, ‘The internet platforms must ban this Islamist stuff, because these people have disgusting views.’ Well, free speech is not about banning disgusting views. It’s about going after them when they’re really dangerous. It’s about arguing with them. But the idea that just because they seem ‘disgusting’ to Theresa May is sufficient reason for them to be taken down by the internet platforms suggests she really hasn’t got the basic idea of free speech.

She needs to read your book.

And I hope she will soon have the leisure to do so.

I’ve noticed that quite a few tweets from people I follow don’t show anymore, but say: ‘This media may contain sensitive material.’ Why is that? Has something changed?

It very much has. One characteristic of this connected world is that we now have a global public sphere provided by a few private companies. There’s the notion of ‘POPS’—Privately Owned Public Spaces. Increasingly we’re getting our news from Facebook, not just exchanging family photos. So they have to face up to the fact that they have some public responsibilities. Then the question is, so how should they do that? What should be the rules of the game? One has to acknowledge that women and people from minorities do get massively harassed and bullied on Facebook and on Twitter and that’s a serious problem. They do have to provide us with the tools to defend ourselves.

“Free speech is not about banning disgusting views. It’s about going after them when they’re really dangerous”

That’s how I would put it, that I should be able to decide the level of privacy I want. You see, free speech is the right of both the speaker and the listener. I should be free to say what I want or not. The listener should be free to hear what he or she wants or not. So I should be able to block people on Twitter.

Now the problem arises—when I was talking to a human rights activist just a couple of days ago in Montreal, she was saying she got 16,000 tweets in a couple of days attacking her on one issue. Well, she can’t spend the day blocking 10,000 accounts. So there are problems of scale, but the principle is very simple. I should be able to decide.

How do you set about increasing civility on the internet? Maybe it’s again something that needs to be taught in school. Just as you’re taught to be polite and say, ‘how do you do,’ when you meet someone, maybe you should be taught that when you disagree on the internet, you shouldn’t launch into personal invective.

Education is incredibly important here. Going back to the navigation metaphor, we really are like people who are steering paddle boats around the lake and suddenly we’re on the high seas. That involves knowing about how you protect your privacy online, how you report really bad stuff, how to keep it away, how to find the good stuff, how to distinguish fake news from true news, but also how you engage with people. I quote in the book a technique, which I love, called ‘constructive controversy.’

I saw this in action in an academy school in east Oxford. Let’s take a controversial subject, the burka say. What’s your position on this? Ahmed argues for this position, and Joe argues for that position. That’s fine. Then you say, ‘Okay, now you have to swap and you have to make the opposite case.’ When that happens, you almost see a sort of light bulb going on in their eyes as they see, ‘Yes, I can imagine what it would be like to see it from their point of view.’

Support Five Books

Five Books interviews are expensive to produce. If you're enjoying this interview, please support us by donating a small amount .

Your next book is: Giving Offense: Essays on Censorship (1997) by the South African Nobel Prize winner J M Coetzee . Quite an interesting collection of essays, isn’t it? He seems not to have made up his mind.

The reason I chose this is that so much of the literature on free speech is either law or philosophy or politics. Here is a writer, a very fine writer, going at it through literature, and what is literature about if not free speech? How we use language, how we interact. What’s so interesting about it is that he looks at the mental state of being offended, and he looks at it with examples from Dostoevsky and Dickens and elsewhere. He says the state of being offended is always the mark of someone who’s unsure of their own position, it’s a mark of weakness. It’s the mark of the bully and the buffoon, and I think that’s a very powerful insight. In saying, ‘I am offended,’ you’re revealing something about yourself.

“The state of being offended is the mark of someone who’s unsure of their own position”

If I extend that story and think about South Africa, I don’t think Nelson Mandela ever took offence—although heaven knows he had reason to be offended by the treatment he was receiving. He maintained his dignity. His position was, ‘You’re the people who are being diminished by this, you’re the people who are losing your dignity, not me.’ This goes against the grain of much contemporary western society, as one sees it in universities and elsewhere, which is almost incitement to take offence. People are being encouraged to take offence at anything they find slightly offensive. Even, for example, Germaine Greer being invited to lecture at a university: ‘I take offence because of her views on trans people.’

So you really feel that in terms of the challenges to free speech, that kind of readiness for everybody to take offence is a problem.

Let’s move to your final pick, The Master Switch  (2010) by Tim Wu. Why should we read this book?

In this completely transformed world of the internet, you have so many different subjects. It’s not just law and philosophy and literature, it’s also computers and it’s also very much about business. Tim Wu is an American ‘cyberlawyer’—a new category of human being—and a rather brilliant one. He’s the guy who coined the term ‘net neutrality,’ which we all use now.

The focus is on information businesses. He says that these are a new kind of business, he says, and that one thing we simply didn’t know 20 years ago is that the network effects on the internet are so powerful that within a very short period of time we have private superpowers—these absolutely massive information empires which have fantastic concentrations of power. If Facebook were a country, it would be the largest country on Earth, with 1.9 billion going on to 2 billion regular monthly users.

“If Facebook were a country, it would be the largest country on Earth”

The American constitutional tradition, including the First Amendment, is brilliant at controlling public power. It’s very good at taking on President Donald Trump. It’s amazing how well it’s responded to Trump. But it’s very bad at controlling private power, and the challenge now is as much about private power—Facebook, Google, and Twitter—as it is about public power. I love the way he explores at the intersection of law, politics, engineering and business, how these information empires have developed, and then the question is how should you try to create checks and balances?

And how should you?

It’s a very difficult question and there’s no simple answer to it. We’ve now got to the stage where they’re so big and so rich and have such cash powers that when some very clever Brits develop something called DeepMind, so they buy DeepMind. Or someone clever develops something called WhatsApp, so they buy WhatsApp. They can buy up the competition. I think there has to be a series of answers of which part is definitely anti-trust. These are near-monopolies. I know because I spend a lot of time talking to people high up in these companies—because I spend three months a year in Stanford and therefore Silicon Valley —that’s what they’re really frightened of, because of their dominant position, particularly in Europe.

But also, if you take Facebook’s news feed, they could swing an election. If they really slanted everything that came across on news feeds to favour the Republicans or the Democrats, they could probably swing that election. So actually you have to think of them, as a kind of media power with some sort of media responsibility. On the other hand, I don’t want to see that being done to Google Search, because Google Search is exactly what it says on the tin, and should be what it says on the tin, a place where I can find everything that’s out there according to some criteria of relevance.

But isn’t that their criteria?

Understand that being a news feed, i.e. a news platform, a media platform, is one thing, being a search engine is another. We have to take these things apart, and then say to ourselves, ‘What is it we really want to ask them?’ And be careful what you wish for, because what’s happened with this famous European court ruling on the right to be forgotten is that now, in effect, Google is exercising a kind of arbitrary censorship, taking down hundreds of thousands, even millions, of links in ways which are not transparent, not accountable, not appealable.

In the book, he’s looking at the history as well. He’s saying that when the telephone started—or the telegraph—when all these things started, everybody thought it was all going to be great, but then they end up in the hands of corporates. It’s a sort of cautionary tale. Don’t think because the internet has started all happy and good that it’s going to end up not going down the same route.

It’s interesting. This book is from 2010. That was seen as rather an original thing to say then. In 2017, people think, ‘my god, what do you mean, they’re so wonderful?’ The perception has changed, but if you go back to the 1990s, it really was, ‘The internet will set you free.’ It was this cyber-libertarian poppycock that you find with every new technology, including printing. It’s hailed as a thing that will set people free, and, at the same time, you always have the catastrophists who say it’ll be the end of human civilisation as we know it. Of course the truth is that it’s neither heaven nor hell.

“I think one of the really important things that we need to do at the moment is to try and get our act together as liberals”

But he’s very interesting in his analysis about complex things. There are serious reasons to be worried about some of the stuff they’re doing, but I don’t think the answer to it is to bring in a law against everything, which, particularly in Germany, is what is being proposed at the moment. I spend a lot of time in Germany, and the debate is ‘big, bad American private superpowers’ versus ‘good, virtuous German and European public powers.’ I don’t think that’s the answer actually. We’ll end up over-regulating them and destroying some of the good things that they do.

But anti-trust involves a regulator.

Anti-trust is a really important place for regulation. But with a lot of this stuff—like what do we want them to do on news feeds, or what do we want them to do about hate speech, and so on—you’ll get much farther, in my experience, with a kind of constructive engagement with these companies, because they’re desperately trying to work out what to do. If you look at the world from the Googleplex, or Facebook headquarters, these amazing—I don’t know if you’ve ever been to them, but they make Washington look slightly dowdy—you’re looking round the world and you’re getting competing demands from every side. Everyone is asking something of you, but every demand is different. Even NGOs—free speech NGOs want you to take down less content, women’s rights and minority rights want you to take down more content, so what the hell are you going to do?

I think one of the really important things that we need to do at the moment is to try and get our act together as liberals, in the broader sense within civil society, and say what are the four or five most important things we want Facebook to do, or Google to do, or Twitter to do?

You mentioned IFEX, this network of free speech organisations. Do you feel there’s enough people working on this and thinking about it?

I don’t think there are enough people, because we now suddenly are in this world where you have giant public superpowers, China, America, Europe, and giant private superpowers, but all these relatively small non-governmental organisations that are slightly fragmented and diverse. That makes for creativity and originality and so on, but it doesn’t make for scale. If you’re trying to get attention from Facebook or Google, or indeed from China or the United States, you need a certain scale. That’s a real problem, actually.

Get the weekly Five Books newsletter

Also, I think one has to distinguish between these various platforms. I love Twitter because it’s an explicitly public platform. It is explicitly for public debate. It’s a brilliant way of having public debate. If someone says something really outrageous, stupid, deeply offensive, they get called out on it straight away. I posted some fake news myself.

Someone did a montage of two photos from parliament, the House of Commons. One showed a completely empty chamber, when apparently they were debating social care or something terribly important for millions of people. The other showed a packed chamber, when they were allegedly debating MPs’ pay. I tweeted this. Within five minutes, I had three people come back at me back saying this was fake news, and that the picture of the full chamber was actually them debating student fees, not MPs’ pay. That’s a great example of where social media can actually be used to refute fake news.

Yes, because the problem with Facebook is you’re supposed to be friends. So you might have a friend who believes that vaccinations caused her son’s life-threatening allergies and puts up a lot of anti-vaccination stuff on her feed. You completely disagree, but you don’t feel you can say, “This is nonsense.”

You’ve got it in one. In my book I quote some really good studies which show that there’s much more hate speech on Facebook than on Twitter for that very simple reason, that you’re supposed to be friends. So people don’t call each other out, even if they should. Whereas on Twitter—I think the study was in Kenya, where people were saying really nasty things about other tribes or ethnic groups—they were being called out. So you should go on Twitter. You must be on Twitter?

We are, we are. @Five_Books .

And so am I.  @fromTGA . I love it.

November 10, 2017

Five Books aims to keep its book recommendations and interviews up to date. If you are the interviewee and would like to update your choice of books (or even just what you say about them) please email us at [email protected]

Timothy Garton Ash

Timothy Garton Ash is Professor of European Studies at the University of Oxford and Isaiah Berlin Professorial Fellow at St Antony’s College, where he is also director of Free Speech Debate,  a global research project of the Dahrendorf Programme for the Study of Freedom. You can click on a country to see what's going on there in terms of free speech here.

In addition, he is a Senior Fellow at the Hoover Institution at Stanford University.

We ask experts to recommend the five best books in their subject and explain their selection in an interview.

This site has an archive of more than one thousand seven hundred interviews, or eight thousand book recommendations. We publish at least two new interviews per week.

Five Books participates in the Amazon Associate program and earns money from qualifying purchases.

© Five Books 2024

This Is Study!

  • Professional & Career
  • Early Childhood Education
  • Books, letters, and stories communicate without speech.
  • February 04, 2017, 11:30:16 PM
  • February 04, 2017, 11:37:07 PM
  • February 04, 2017, 11:50:18 PM
  • International edition
  • Australia edition
  • Europe edition

Jonathan Bryan

Give a fair hearing to people without speech

We are writing to express our disappointment and anger with the article by Christopher French and Michael Marshall about Jonathan Bryan ( Why communication from a ‘locked-in’ child is a miracle we must question , 5 February). After consulting with Jonathan’s mother, who has asked us to focus on the important principles rather than Jonathan’s story, we are writing to express our concerns for the wider group of people without speech.

We are Hardcore AAC, a collective of people who believe in the right to communication for people without biological speech and use augmentative and alternative communication (AAC). Our active Facebook group has 459 members. This group includes disabled people without speech as well as those with speech, parents of disabled people without speech, teachers and speech and language therapists. We are writing this letter as a group but the principal signatories are all disabled people without speech who use and need other forms of communication.

Firstly, we believe the article is a harmful representation of AAC users. Media representation for AAC users is often inaccurate and potentially damaging, and this article merely reinforces the prejudices we face. For the majority of your readership who may not be aware of AAC and have little knowledge of communication impairments apart from Stephen Hawking, it does not help to read an article which questions our methods of communicating.

The article goes in to great length and detail about facilitated communication (FC), when Jonathan does not use FC. Reading this article one could easily be mistaken that Jonathan too uses FC. The method he uses is a simple eye-pointing technique using an E-Tran frame, a widely available low-tech communication aid. It can be used with minimal training. E-Tran is widely used by people without speech and is accepted by senior speech therapists. It has even been used by people taking their GCSEs and is accepted by examination boards.

There are many more stories like Jonathan’s and there are many more people without communication who should be given the benefit of the doubt, and need people to believe in them. We are used to having our accomplishments questioned, rarely acknowledged or celebrated. It does not help to have an article written by someone who has not even met Jonathan. Our group will be happy to meet the authors to demonstrate what those without speech can do.

Kate Caryer AAC user and playwright Jamie Preece AAC user Jemima Hughes AAC user and filmmaker Marlena May Katene AAC user and journalist Adam Lenartowicz AAC user and undergraduate Simon Stevens AAC user and disability consultant Rosie Vachat AAC user and musician Patrick Bates AAC user Afonso Matos Ramalhoso AAC user Sam Sillars AAC user and gap year student Marion Stanton Teacher supporting students who rely on AAC Jenny Herd Parent of an AAC user at university Matthew Williams Father and accessible technology blogger Samantha Green Registered trainee sign language interpreter Stephen Eykyn Parent of nine-year-old AAC user Jo Cope Parent of 12-year-old AAC user in mainstream education Cal Desmond Pearson Ally and friend of AAC users Helen Seth Parent of two AAC users Julia Paylor Parent of a communication aid user Janet Mayes Parent of adult communication aid user Al Crabtree Parent of 19-year-old AAC user Seray Ibrahim Speech and language therapist and PhD student Kate McCallum Sibling of an AAC user Midge Caryer Parent of an AAC user Chloe Kazantzis Friend of AAC users Emma Sullivan Wife of AAC user Maggie Robson Actor and friend of AAC user Katie Clarke Parent of AAC user Dr Hilary Gardner Speech and language therapist Katherine Mitchell Learning disability centre manager Anna Jeronymides Parent of AAC user Julie Bello Parent of AAC user for 25-plus years Caroline Regan Actor and friend of AAC user Sarah Ismail Disabled blogger Daisy Clay Speech and language therapist Jules Whicher Parent of AAC user Megan Glynn Speech and language therapist Rachel Martin Parent and chair of Assert (Angelman Syndrome Support Education and Research Trust) Kayleigh Millar Disabled person Andrew Little Disabled person Kate Jackson Parent of AAC user Katie Fraser Disabled person Sandra Barrett Parent of AAC user Hester Hughes Works in education Clare Millington Parent of AAC user Gerard Mulgrew Parent of AAC user Rebecca Philips Becky Ryan Liz Beattie Mary Regan Jeffrey Benge

Join the debate – email [email protected]

  • Special educational needs

Most viewed

IMAGES

  1. Alphabet Chart For Non Verbal Communication

    books letters and stories communicate without speech

  2. 5 Ways to Communicate Without Talking

    books letters and stories communicate without speech

  3. Considerate Classroom: Early Childhood Special Education Edition

    books letters and stories communicate without speech

  4. 11 Children's Books to Teach Letter Writing to Kids

    books letters and stories communicate without speech

  5. 5 ways to communicate without talking

    books letters and stories communicate without speech

  6. Wordless Picture Books + FREE Downloadable List

    books letters and stories communicate without speech

VIDEO

  1. HOW TO COMMUNICATE WITHOUT SPEECH. #PHANEROO #APOSTLE GRACE LUBEGA

  2. I CAN..........Learn to Read!

  3. 9 Books That Made Me An Articulate Speaker

  4. Never touch a crocodile book reading quiet for little ears

  5. Capital Letter tracing R

  6. The Book With No Pictures By B.J. Novak read aloud

COMMENTS

  1. ECED 230 CH16 Flashcards

    Language acquisition is a sequential process moving from simple to complex. Crying is one of the first attempts at communication. Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like Language is a complicated system of symbols and sounds., Books, letters, and stories communicate without speech., The constructivists propose that ...

  2. Promoting Language with Books

    In addition, it's easy for children to show their parents what interests them in a book, both with and without words (by pointing, gesturing, or drawing attention to a picture). Books motivate children to communicate and, when parents respond to what the child is interested in, it helps the child learn new words. 3.

  3. Teaching children with nonverbal autism to read

    2. Nonverbal interactive reading. As you read with your child, give him opportunities to interact nonverbally. Here are some activities you can share as you read: Run your finger just under the text as you read. Then ask your child do the pointing. Ask your child to turn the pages at the right time.

  4. Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC): What to know

    Highlights. Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) is a powerful tool that can transform the lives of people who have difficulty with speech. AAC is especially important for autistic children, because it gives them a way to communicate without speech. AAC can ensure your child always has a way to communicate, allowing for fluctuating ...

  5. ISAAC

    By Anne Warrick (1998) ISAAC Series: Volume 1. Communication Without Speech is an introduction to AAC. This book answers commonly asked questions and offers practical tips and illustrations. Topics include communication boards, vocabulary selection, assessment and education. ISAAC Order Form (PDF, 118 kB)

  6. How to Use Books to Promote Communication Skills

    Reading every word in the book is an option but not required to engage your child in a language-rich learning experience. Books that include songs, rhymes, and repetitions are a great way to get your child involved in the story. Have them repeat common phrases, sing-a-long, and predict rhymes throughout the story to keep them engaged.

  7. Communicating Without Speech: Practical Augmentative and Alternative

    Augmentative and alternative communication (AAC) includes gesture and signing, and the use of pictures and symbol charts. Electronic voice synthesizers are also forms of AAC, but ordinary writing and typing are not. The first two chapters of Communicating without Speech deal with the neurology of speech and language disorders.

  8. Encourage Conversations with Wordless Books!

    Encourage Conversations with Wordless Books! By Lauren Lowry Hanen Certified SLP and Clinical Staff Writer . The following statement might surprise you: Reading a book that doesn't have any words is great for a child's language development. You might be wondering how a book without any words could possibly help a child learn about language.

  9. Wordless Picture Books for Speech and Language Therapy

    Here are my top 10 favorite wordless picture books that I use all the time! Chalk by Bill Thomson. Wave by Suzy Lee. The Red Book by Barbara Lehman. Flotsam by David Wiesner. The Lion and the Mouse by Jerry Pinkney. Wolf in the Snow by Matthew Cordell. Hank Finds an Egg by Rebecca Dudley. Journey by Aaron Becker.

  10. Personalized Early AAC Intervention to Build Language and Literacy

    Furthermore, literacy skills may also be beneficial for children with complex communication needs who experience significant challenges in speech production as written language makes explicit the individual speech sounds and their sequence in words through application of letter sound knowledge to the orthographic sequence of letters (Light ...

  11. Best Children's Books for Speech and Language Development

    As your child increases their descriptive repertoire, it can promote additional language to help increase the structure and length of their phrases and sentences. Tactile books to check out: - Bright Baby Touch and Feel ( Words, Colors, Numbers, Shapes) by Roger Pridy. - Feely Bugs by David A. Carter.

  12. Exceptional Child Final Flashcards

    Books, letters, and stories communicate without speech. False. A self-help skill for a 36 month old would be to use all eating utensils neatly. False. Parent well-being has no impact on the development of a child with a disability. True. Chaining is an effective way to teach self-care skills.

  13. ECE200, Exceptional Child, Chapter 16 Quiz Flashcards

    The onset of pre-linguistic communication is about the age of two years. False. Books, letters, and stories communicate without speech. True. In a quality early childhood setting, talking is: encouraged, and children do most of the talking. ECE200, Exceptional Child, Chapter 16 Quiz.

  14. Stories Without Language

    Specifically, the chapter has a twofold aim: showing the plausibility of the existence of stories without language; showing that narrative communication is the evolutionary precondition for the emergence of language. After analyzing why (the evolutionary and functional reasons) humans invented language, the last step concerns how they could do so.

  15. Communication Without Speech

    Guide for caregivers of children with temporary or permanent speech difficulties. Authors from diverse backgrounds discuss early communication, the role of the family and professionals, and the use of technology. Includes a bibliography and extensive guide to speech services and resources in Australia.

  16. Non Verbal Communication Books

    avg rating 3.63 — 915 ratings — published 2010. Want to Read. Rate this book. 1 of 5 stars 2 of 5 stars 3 of 5 stars 4 of 5 stars 5 of 5 stars. Books shelved as non-verbal-communication: What Every Body is Saying: An Ex-FBI Agent's Guide to Speed-Reading People by Joe Navarro, Emotions Revealed: ...

  17. Storytelling is a powerful communication tool

    Developed by TED-Ed, the course also features a library of full-length TED Talks from Brené Brown, Bryan Stevenson, Susan Cain and others to reinforce its lessons. In this post, which is adapted from the TED Masterclass app and his book, Anderson discusses how we can learn to use storytelling to elevate our speeches, presentations and talks.

  18. Books about Without Speech

    Similar Novels Related Stories 86% Dreaming of a Speech Without Words: the Paintings and Early Objects of H. C. Westermann Michael et al (text) Rooks

  19. The best books on Free Speech

    Interview by Sophie Roell, Editor. Free Speech: Ten Principles for a Connected World. by Timothy Garton Ash. Read. 1 On Liberty by John Stuart Mill. 2 How to Cure A Fanatic by Amos Oz. 3 Defending My Enemy: American Nazis, the Skokie Case, and the Risks of Freedom by Aryeh Neier. 4 Giving Offense: Essays on Censorship by J M Coetzee.

  20. ch 16 study guide Flashcards

    Preview. Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like children learn to talk by imitating words and then receiving feedback (reinforcement) for saying them is from the environmental perspective, books, letters, and stories communicate without speech, children must first coo and babble before they can use words and sentences ...

  21. (Answered) Books, letters, and stories communicate without speech

    Books, letters, and stories communicate without speech. a. True b. False Indicate whether the statement is true or false

  22. Give a fair hearing to people without speech

    The method he uses is a simple eye-pointing technique using an E-Tran frame, a widely available low-tech communication aid. It can be used with minimal training. E-Tran is widely used by people ...

  23. CHURCHES OF CHRIST NATIONAL LECTURESHIP

    THE CONTEXT AND APPLICATION OF SCRIPTURE: Bro., Dr. James Michael Crusoe, Arlington Rd Church of Christ, Hopewell, VA

  24. Books, leters, and stories communicate without speech

    Required Materials Word processing software Step 1: Preparation In this activity, you will write a. The Woman Of Hidden Figures what evidence from the book supports the central idea. The report is due on Wednesday, but we're flying back from Miami that morning. O Run-on O Correct Is this sentence a run on.